This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

proofread

We're more likely to break conservation rules when we see others doing it, scientists say

We're more likely to break conservation rules when we see others doing it too
Here, an individual fisher (A) has their imitation response triggered (motor processes) as two fishers move past them and a sign indicating a Marine Protected Area (MPA) where fishing is not allowed (fishing is prohibited in the area on the right-hand side of the broken line, and the curved red arrow indicates the movement of the two fishers moving inside the MPA to start fishing). (B) Their gaze is drawn to two fishers acting in non-compliance by harvesting inside the boundaries of the MPA (perceptual and attentional processes—the observing fisher's field of view is shown in the red shaded area). (C) The fisher evaluates a specific situation of non-compliance where their attitude towards engagement changes (evaluative process), and in (D) fishers are seen breaking a rule and the rule-breaking behavior is more readily attributed to the rule breakers wider representative group (i.e., all local fishers) than when fishers are seen complying with the rule (asymmetrical learning rates)—shown here as red sinuous lines—the more a behavior is readily attributed to all group members the thicker the line is. Credit: Arlidge et al.

Seeing other people break rules can make us act out in ways that we wouldn't on our own. Applying this psychological concept to fields like conservation could help explain why social influence causes people to make decisions that are harmful to the environment.

By synthesizing insights from the fields of cognitive psychology, microsociology, and in an opinion article publishing on September 13 in the journal Trends in Ecology & Evolution, a group of scientists, ecologists, and demonstrate the role that social influence plays when people decide to break conservation rules and suggest ways to leverage this knowledge to better protect our and ecosystems.

"Rules that govern are a key component of biodiversity conservation," write the researchers, who are based in Germany and Chile. "Reducing and preventing non-compliance with conservation-related rules can contribute to securing livelihoods, , public safety, and the conservation of biodiversity."

Conservation rules can look different depending on the environmental concerns in the region they're aimed at protecting. But in their article, the authors point to research showing that conservation rules are regularly broken. They also note that when rule-followers see others breaking rules, as research shows is relatively common, they might be more likely to engage in rule-breaking behaviors themselves.

"Consider a fisher who is thinking that people typically do not break the rule of no fishing inside their local no-take protected area," write the researchers. "This fisher might also assume that other people would not approve of people breaking the rule. In the immediate situation, however, the fisher might act in non-compliance of the rule because they see several other fishers who are fishing inside a no-take protected area and they are not getting caught."

The team argues that rule-breaking behaviors can be harmful for both the species and ecosystems the rules are protecting and for the rule-breakers themselves. "Even minor infractions can ultimately result in severe outcomes when people imitate the behavior of other rule breakers. For example, hikers leaving waymarked trails in protected areas can lead to littering, trampling of protected flora, fauna, and fungi, and occasionally human deaths," they write.

While it's true that some people might be more naturally inclined to break certain rules than others, the authors state, a person's predisposition can't fully explain rule-breaking behaviors. "What happens at the moment of decision-making, including interactions among , their surrounding environment, and ensuing social identity changes, play an essential role in both individual and group behavior," says the team.

In order to improve the way we think about, implement, and enforce conservation interventions, the authors argue that we need to learn more about how and why people are breaking rules. They suggest that we experimentally test how social influences can impact people's decisions using technology such as wearable electronics and virtual reality devices. For example, researchers could see whether people stray from a waymarked path while walking alone or around others venturing into areas where leaving the path is forbidden.

"Testing situational processes that can lead to non-compliant behaviors presents clear benefits," write the authors. "Spreading dynamics of rule-breaking behaviors in groups can be identified. This understanding can then support applied non-compliance research to improve the framing, development, and implementation of conservation interventions."

More information: William N.S. Arlidge et al, Situational social influence leading to non-compliance with conservation rules, Trends in Ecology & Evolution (2023). DOI: 10.1016/j.tree.2023.08.003

Journal information: Trends in Ecology & Evolution

Provided by Cell Press

Citation: We're more likely to break conservation rules when we see others doing it, scientists say (2023, August 29) retrieved 27 April 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2023-08-scientists.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Study: Children as young as 5 are quite discerning when it comes to cheaters

103 shares

Feedback to editors