In mongoose society, immigrants are a bonus—when given time to settle in

In mongoose society, immigrants are a bonus -- when given time to settle in
Dwarf mongoose sentinels on guard duty. Credit: Shannon Benson

Researchers studying wild dwarf mongooses have provided insight into what happens when immigrants join a new group. The study in Current Biology on December 4 shows that, initially, recent immigrants rarely serve as lookout, which means they provide little information to help the rest of the group. Even when they do act cooperatively, their new groupmates tend to ignore what they have to offer. But, within five months, the new arrivals become fully integrated and valued members within mongoose society.

"A few months after arriving in a new group, former immigrants are contributing as often as residents and their information is used just as much, but to reach that stage requires a transition period," says Andrew Radford of the University of Bristol, UK.

That's probably because the dispersal process is tough on individuals, notes the lead author of the study, Julie Kern. "Recent immigrants are typically exhausted and run down, as evidenced by a loss of weight," she says. "Even if they tried, they couldn't contribute fully at first because other members don't yet know them."

The study is one of the first to explore the impact of dispersal events on provision and use within groups. It also shows that the expected benefits associated with an increase in may not be as straightforward as scientists have often assumed.

Dwarf mongooses within a group depend on each other. For instance, individuals will stand guard, serving as sentinels that keep a watch for predators and call to warn the rest of the group when one is near. Sentinels also issue calls to announce that they're watching. By providing this social information, sentinels allow other group members to concentrate more on foraging. What the researchers wanted to know is how new group members contributed to these tasks and whether the information they provided was used by others.

This video shows dwarf mongoose sentinels on duty. Credit: Julie Kern

Kern watched nine groups of wild dwarf mongooses living on Sorabi Rock Lodge, Limpopo Province, South Africa, for years, beginning in 2011. The groups, ranging in size from 7 to 12 individuals, have been habituated to the close presence of people. They are even trained to climb onto a scale to weigh themselves. Researchers can also identify the individuals based on small blonde dye marks or distinctive physical markings.

Of the 165 individually marked dwarf mongooses in the population, 35 dispersed into or out of the groups under observation. The researchers recorded dispersal events in all nine of the groups, with a total of 22 emigrations and 28 immigrations witnessed.

Kern and Radford found from the data that new immigrants rarely served as sentinels within the first month of arrival. But, by the time they'd been in their new group for five months, they acted in that role just as often as longer-term residents did. The researchers suggest that this shift in behavior might be a result of the challenges associated with dispersal.

Using a series of experiments in which they played back previously recorded calls, the showed that foragers responded significantly less to the surveillance calls of recent immigrants compared to those from resident group members. When they heard the call of a new , they continued to spend more time looking around for predators themselves. But, those differences in response to new members also disappeared after a few months.

This video shows dwarf mongooses foraging. Credit: Julie Kern

The findings show that an integration period is necessary after dispersers have joined a new group before the benefits of an increased group size can be fully realized. The dispersers need a period in which to recover before being able to contribute fully to cooperative duty, and residents need time to learn about new group members before relying on their social information.

"Given a chance, immigrants can become valued members of mongoose society," Radford says. "The increased group size that results is beneficial to all."

Kern says they will continue to study the mongooses' sentinel behavior in the wider context of cooperation within the species. "For example," she says, "we'd like to know whether the behavior is traded for other beneficial acts."

More information: Current Biology, Kern and Radford: "Reduced social information provision by immigrants and use by residents following dispersal" www.cell.com/current-biology/f … 0960-9822(17)31382-9 , DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.045

Journal information: Current Biology

Provided by Cell Press

Citation: In mongoose society, immigrants are a bonus—when given time to settle in (2017, December 4) retrieved 20 June 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2017-12-mongoose-society-immigrants-bonuswhen.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Mongoose sentinels respond flexibly to threats

7 shares

Feedback to editors