This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

proofread

Brittle stars can learn just fine, even without a brain

Brittle stars can learn just fine—even without a brain
In a series of experiments, brittle stars learned that "lights out" was a dinner bell call to come for dinner. Credit: Julia Notar

We humans are fixated on big brains as a proxy for smarts. But headless animals called brittle stars have no brains at all and still manage to learn through experience, new research reveals.

Relatives of starfish, brittle stars spend most of their time hiding under rocks and crevices in the ocean or burrowing in the sand.

These shy marine creatures have no brain to speak of—just nerve cords running down each of their five wiggly arms, which join to form a nerve ring near their mouth.

"There's no processing center," said lead author Julia Notar, who did the research as part of her biology Ph.D. in professor Sönke Johnsen's lab at Duke University.

"Each of the nerve cords can act independently," Notar said. "It's like instead of a boss, there's a committee."

In the case of brittle stars, that seems to be enough to learn by association, Notar, Johnsen and former Duke undergraduate Madeline Go report titled "Learning Without a Brain: Classical Conditioning in the Ophiuroid Ophiocoma echinata," in the journal Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

This type of learning involves associating different stimuli via a process called classical conditioning. A famous example is Pavlov's dog experiments, which showed that dogs repeatedly fed at the ringing of a bell would eventually start drooling at the mere sound of a bell, even when no food was around.

Humans do this all the time. If you hear the "ding" of a smartphone over and over again with each new alert, eventually the sound starts to have a special meaning. Just hearing someone's phone ping or buzz with the same chime as yours is enough to make you reflexively reach for your own phone in anticipation of the next text, email, or Instagram post.

Classical conditioning has been demonstrated in a handful of previous studies in starfish. But most echinoderms—a group of some 7,000 species that includes brittle stars and similarly brainless starfish, and sea cucumbers—have not been tested.

To find out if brittle stars are capable of learning, the researchers put 16 black brittle stars (Ophiocoma echinata) in individual water tanks and used a video camera to record their behavior.

This time-lapse video shows a classical conditioning experiment Duke researchers conducted to see if brittle stars – which don’t have brains – could learn. Every time the lights went dim, the researchers put a pipette with a morsel of shrimp in the animals’ tanks. Over time the animals learned that “lights out” was a dinner bell call to come for dinner. Credit: Duke University

Half the brittle stars were trained by dimming the lights for 30 minutes whenever the animals were fed. Every time the lights went out, the researchers would put a morsel of shrimp—"which they love"—in the tanks, placed just out of reach.

The other half got just as much shrimp and also experienced a 30-minute dark period, but never at the same time—the animals were fed under lit conditions.

Whether it was or dark, the animals spent most of their time hiding behind the filters in their tanks; only coming out at mealtime. But only the trained brittle stars learned to associate darkness with food.

Early in the 10-month-long experiment, the animals stayed hidden when the lights went out. But over time, the animals made such a connection between the darkness and mealtime that they reacted as if food was on its way and crept out of hiding whenever the lights went out, even before any food was put in the tanks.

These brittle stars had learned a new association: lights out meant that food was likely to show up. They didn't need to smell or taste the shrimp to react. Just sensing the lights go dim was enough to make them come when called for dinner.

They still remembered the lesson even after a 13-day 'break' without training, i.e., dimming the lights over and over again without feeding them.

Notar said the results are "exciting" because "classical conditioning hasn't really been shown definitively in this group of animals before."

"Knowing that can learn means they're not just robotic scavengers like little Roombas cleaning up the ocean floor," Notar said. "They're potentially able to expect and avoid predators or anticipate food because they're learning about their environment."

As a next step, Notar hopes to start to tease apart how they manage to learn and remember using a that is so different from our own.

"People ask me all the time, 'how do they do it?'" Notar said. "We don't know yet. But I hope to have more answers in a few years."

More information: Julia C. Notar et al, Learning without a brain: classical conditioning in the ophiuroid Ophiocoma echinata, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (2023). DOI: 10.1007/s00265-023-03402-x

Provided by Duke University

Citation: Brittle stars can learn just fine, even without a brain (2023, November 30) retrieved 28 April 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2023-11-brittle-stars-fine-brain.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Oldest known samples of brittle stars from supercontinent Gondwana discovered in South Africa

59 shares

Feedback to editors