This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

written by researcher(s)

proofread

Bees are astonishingly good at making decisions—and our computer model explains how that's possible

Bees are astonishingly good at making decisions—and our computer model explains how that's possible
Credit: AI-generated image (disclaimer)

A honey bee's life depends on it successfully harvesting nectar from flowers to make honey. Deciding which flower is most likely to offer nectar is incredibly difficult.

Getting it right demands correctly weighing up subtle cues on flower type, age and history—the best indicators a flower might contain a tiny drop of nectar. Getting it wrong is at best a waste of time, and at worst means exposure to a lethal predator hiding in the .

In new research published today in eLife our team reports how bees make these .

A field of artificial flowers

We challenged bees with a field of artificial flowers made from colored disks of card, each of which offered a tiny drop of sugar syrup. Different-colored "flowers" varied in their likelihood of offering sugar, and also differed in how well bees could judge whether or not the fake flower offered a reward.

We put tiny, harmless paint marks on the back of each bee, and filmed every visit a bee made to the flower array. We then used and machine learning to automatically extract the position and flight path of the bee. From this information, we could assess and precisely time every single decision the bees made.

We found bees very quickly learned to identify the most rewarding flowers. They quickly assessed whether to accept or reject a flower, but perplexingly their correct choices were on average faster (0.6 seconds) than their incorrect choices (1.2 seconds).

This is the opposite of what we expected.

Usually in animals—and even in artificial systems—an accurate decision takes longer than an inaccurate decision. This is called the speed-accuracy tradeoff.

This happens because determining whether a decision is right or wrong usually depends on how much evidence we have to make that decision. More evidence means we can make a more accurate decision—but gathering evidence takes time. So accurate decisions are usually slow and inaccurate decisions are faster.

The speed-accuracy tradeoff occurs so often in engineering, psychology and biology, you could almost call it a "law of psychophysics". And yet bees seemed to be breaking this law.

The only other animals known to beat the speed-accuracy tradeoff are humans and primates.

How then can a bee, with its tiny yet remarkable brain, be performing on a par with primates?

Bees avoid risk

To take apart this question we turned to a , asking what properties a system would need to have to beat the speed-accuracy tradeoff.

We built capable of processing , learning and making decisions. We compared the performance of these artificial decision systems to the real bees. From this we could identify what a system had to have if it were to beat the tradeoff.

The answer lay in giving "accept" and "reject" responses different time-bound evidence thresholds. Here's what that means—bees only accepted a flower if, at a glance, they were sure it was rewarding. If they had any uncertainty, they rejected it.

This was a risk-averse strategy and meant bees might have missed some rewarding flowers, but it successfully focused their efforts only on the flowers with the best chance and best evidence of providing them with sugar.

Our computer model of how bees were making fast, accurate decisions mapped well to both their behavior and the known pathways of the bee brain.

Our model is plausible for how are such effective and fast decision makers. What's more, it gives us a template for how we might build systems—such as autonomous robots for exploration or mining—with these features.

More information: HaDi MaBouDi et al, How honey bees make fast and accurate decisions, eLife (2023). DOI: 10.7554/eLife.86176

Journal information: eLife

Provided by The Conversation

This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.The Conversation

Citation: Bees are astonishingly good at making decisions—and our computer model explains how that's possible (2023, July 1) retrieved 27 April 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2023-06-bees-astonishingly-good-decisionsand.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Honey bees make rapid, accurate decisions and could inspire future of AI, study suggests

64 shares

Feedback to editors