Desert ants' survival strategy emerges from millions of simple interactions

February 18, 2019 by Molly Sharlach, Princeton University
Princeton researchers have created a mathematical model to explain how desert harvester ants collectively weigh the cost of losing water while foraging against the benefit of bringing in more food. Credit: Renato Pagliara Vasquez

Ants' frenzied movements may seem aimless and erratic to a casual observer, but closer study reveals that an ant colony's collective behavior can help it thrive in a harsh environment and may also yield inspiration for robotic systems.

In a new analysis, Princeton researchers have created a to explain how desert harvester ants collectively weigh the cost of losing water while foraging against the benefit of bringing in more food. The model is a tool for investigating how respond to a changing environment, and how behavioral differences among colonies affect their long-term survival and reproductive success. The team published their results Dec. 4, 2018, in the journal PLOS Computational Biology.

Professor Naomi Ehrich Leonard, the study's senior author, has previously analyzed the dynamics of bird flocks and fish schools to better understand how large groups can operate efficiently without central control. She is a leader in a multidisciplinary approach at Princeton to the field of robotics and cyberphysical systems in service to society. Insights gained from these can help in the design of robot teams to carry out search and rescue missions or take measurements of environments inaccessible to humans.

A few years ago, when Leonard met Stanford University biologist Deborah Gordon at a research conference, she was intrigued by the prospect of examining the behavior of harvester ants, which Gordon's team has been tracking for the past three decades at a field site in the New Mexico desert.

"It was this beautiful marriage of the opportunity not just to collect data, but to define experiments—to use our models and our perspective to try to understand the connection between what individuals are doing and what happens at the level of the group," said Leonard, Princeton's Edwin S. Wilsey Professor of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering.

The ants offer an excellent system for investigating how a group of interacting individuals makes tradeoffs in uncertain conditions. In the hot, dry deserts of the southwestern United States and northern Mexico, red harvester ants (Pogonomyrmex barbatus) rely on seeds for both food and water. But leaving the nest to gather seeds is a risky business: A colony must collectively weigh the cost of losing water while foraging against the benefit of bringing in more seeds.

"The ants are able to regulate the rate at which they send out foragers with a very limited communication framework" that uses their sense of smell, said graduate student Renato Pagliara Vasquez, the study's lead author. When two ants tap their antennae together, he explained, "they can smell what are called cuticular hydrocarbons, and that smell changes when they've been outside the nest. One ant can also tell if the other is carrying a seed, and this information is enough to regulate the entire foraging behavior of the colony."

Pagliara Vasquez and Leonard worked with Gordon to develop a model that connects these interactions to the likelihood that a potential forager will leave the nest in search of seeds, allowing them to analyze how a colony's foraging rates respond to environmental conditions.

Princeton graduate student Renato Pagliara Vasquez spent part of two summers conducting field research on desert harvester ants in southern New Mexico. Credit: Renato Pagliara Vasquez

To gather the data that informed the model, Pagliara Vasquez spent part of two summers with Gordon's team at their study site in southern New Mexico, just east of the Arizona border. The researchers used videos and computer-vision software, as well as manual counts, to record 13 colonies of ants entering and exiting their nests during the morning hours, before it got too hot for the ants to forage. By 11 a.m., soil surface temperatures sometimes exceeded 120 degrees Fahrenheit (49 degrees Celsius), with relative humidity dropping to 20 percent, in contrast to the moist, 80-degree (27 degrees Celsius) conditions inside the ants' nests.

These measurements allowed the researchers to compute each colony's incoming and outgoing foraging rates. They also tracked how these rates changed over the course of a morning and how foraging dynamics differed among colonies.

The team studied foraging behavior as a "closed-loop system" in which the environment and the colony's foraging activities outside the nest influence the ants' interactions inside the nest, which in turn affect further foraging. Interactions with ants returning from foraging influence whether an ant inside the nest will go out to forage. The researchers are interested in how ants' sensitivity to these signals shifts with changing conditions.

The researchers call this sensitivity level volatility, a measure of an implicit threshold of interactions required to influence an ant to forage. The concept is inspired by neuroscience. Similar patterns of excitability allow collections of neurons to carry out complex tasks such as memory and movement through many interactions between individual neurons.

"Researchers in the field have previously drawn broad analogies between populations of neurons and populations of social insects, but either at the level of behavior, or patterns of interaction," said James Marshall, a professor of theoretical and computational biology at the University of Sheffield in the U.K., who was not involved in the study. "In this case the researchers have really pushed the analogy down to the level of describing individual ant behavior as if they were single neurons, and shown how this can result in sophisticated colony-level behavior."

"The ants don't know what the current temperature or humidity is outside the nest, so they become informed the first time they leave the nest," explained Pagliara Vasquez. "So, we proposed this very simple mechanism, which is very powerful: Once they've been outside for the first time they change how sensitive they are to interactions with returning foragers. In essence, the colony can use the accumulated information from the incoming ants to regulate how sensitive the colony is to sending out new foragers."

The researchers are continuing to examine what underlies the ants' volatility by closely analyzing the behavior of single ants throughout the day. Gordon also plans to integrate the foraging model with years of genetic data on the ant colonies to explore whether behavior that helps the ants conserve water is heritable, since it is known to affect a colony's reproductive success.

"This model puts together the interactions of ants inside the nest and the rate at which they forage outside into one system, so that we can understand the process that evolution is shaping," said Gordon. "Natural selection is acting on how this all works dynamically, and now we have a way to describe that. It's a very elegant way to think about a lot of noisy dynamics and put it together into a model that can be used to guide further work."

Explore further: Feeding ants dopamine might make them smarter foragers

More information: Renato Pagliara et al. Regulation of harvester ant foraging as a closed-loop excitable system, PLOS Computational Biology (2018). DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006200

Related Stories

Feeding ants dopamine might make them smarter foragers

September 27, 2018

In an ant colony, few tasks are as important as gathering food. But the desert heat can pose a challenge for an ant on foraging duty. Recent findings, publishing in the journal iScience on September 27, show how dopamine ...

Evolution shapes new rules for ant behavior

May 15, 2013

In ancient Greece, the city-states that waited until their own harvest was in before attacking and destroying a rival community's crops often experienced better long-term success.

Researchers discover the 'anternet'

August 27, 2012

(Phys.org)—On the surface, ants and the Internet don't seem to have much in common. But two Stanford researchers have discovered that a species of harvester ants determine how many foragers to send out of the nest in much ...

Desert ants have an amazing odor memory

September 24, 2018

Scientists at the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology used behavioral experiments to show that desert ants quickly learn many food odors and remember them for the rest of their lives. However, their memory for nest ...

Recommended for you

Machine learning identifies links between world's oceans

March 21, 2019

Oceanographers studying the physics of the global ocean have long found themselves facing a conundrum: Fluid dynamical balances can vary greatly from point to point, rendering it difficult to make global generalizations.

How fluid viscosity affects earthquake intensity

March 21, 2019

Fault zones play a key role in shaping the deformation of the Earth's crust. All of these zones contain fluids, which heavily influence how earthquakes propagate. In an article published today in Nature Communications, Chiara ...

1 comment

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

Burn one
5 / 5 (1) Feb 19, 2019
The more we learn about ants, the more we find out just how sophisticated they really are. They also adjust forging behavior to adjust for predation. On a side note, I was collecting some of these ants a few decades ago and got stung. This was about the most painful sting I've ever experienced. It felt like a nail had been driven into my foot. The pain lasted 14 hours!

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.