New study strengthens olfactory vibration-sensing theory

Jan 29, 2013 by Bob Yirka report

(Phys.org)—A new study by a team of chemists in Greece has added credence to a theory that suggests that humans differentiate smells by sensing molecular vibrations, rather than through simple binding to receptors. In their paper published in the journal PLoS ONE, the researchers found that deuterating a compound with more hydrogen atoms than had been tested in previous research, resulted in test subjects being able to distinguish between two nearly identical molecules with two distinct vibration frequencies.

For several years there has been a debate in the field regarding how humans distinguish between smells. Prior to the debate, the consensus had been that molecules in compounds bind to receptors in the nose and respond according to fit – rather like pieces of a . That theory was challenged by a team of Greek researchers, led by Luca Turin who claimed that molecule shape alone could not account for differences in smell perception – they suggested in 1996 that people distinguish smells through a process of quantum tunneling whereby molecular vibrations allow electrons to tunnel between .

Turin et al's ideas were delivered a blow in 2004 when another team of researchers in the U.S. tested the theory on live human subjects. They asked volunteers to sniff samples of deuterated (swapping out the in a molecule for to change the frequency of the vibration) and undeuterated acetophenone to see if they could tell the difference between compounds that were nearly identical except for their vibrations. They team found the volunteers were not able to detect a difference. Then, in 2011, Turin and his team showed that fruit-flies were able to differentiate between the very same compounds, suggesting their vibration sensing abilities were sharper than humans.

In this new effort, the researchers replicated the experiment run by the rival team in 2004, and found nearly identical results. But because of their success with the fruit-flies, they theorized that perhaps the problem was with the compound being tested. Acetophenone has just 8 hydrogen atoms. They ran the experiment again, only this time used cyclopentadecanone, which has 28 hydrogen atoms. This time, they report that the human volunteers were able to tell the difference. And this they say, adds weight to their theory of .

Both sides agree that the final proof of which side is correct will only come about when someone devises a means to study the process of odor identification up close – close enough to actually watch the binding that occurs between molecules and sensors, or sees tunneling going on.

Explore further: Pterostilbene, a molecule similar to resveratrol, as a potential treatment for obesity

More information: Gane S, Georganakis D, Maniati K, Vamvakias M, Ragoussis N, et al. (2013) Molecular Vibration-Sensing Component in Human Olfaction. PLoS ONE 8(1): e55780. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0055780

Abstract
Whether olfaction recognizes odorants by their shape, their molecular vibrations, or both remains an open and controversial question. A convenient way to address it is to test for odor character differences between deuterated and undeuterated odorant isotopomers, since these have identical ground-state conformations but different vibrational modes. In a previous paper (Franco et al. (2011) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:9, 3797-802) we showed that fruit flies can recognize the presence of deuterium in odorants by a vibrational mechanism. Here we address the question of whether humans too can distinguish deuterated and undeuterated odorants. A previous report (Keller and Vosshall (2004) Nat Neurosci 7:4, 337-8) indicated that naive subjects are incapable of distinguishing acetophenone and d-8 acetophenone. Here we confirm and extend those results to trained subjects and gas-chromatography [GC]-pure odorants. However, we also show that subjects easily distinguish deuterated and undeuterated musk odorants purified to GC-pure standard. These results are consistent with a vibrational component in human olfaction.

Press release

Related Stories

Quantum explanation for how we smell gets new support

Mar 28, 2011

(PhysOrg.com) -- Since 1996, when biophysicist Luca Turin first suggested that quantum mechanics may help explain how we smell various odors, the idea has met with controversy. In the past 15 years, some studies ...

Fruit flies can detect heavy hydrogen: study

Feb 16, 2011

(PhysOrg.com) -- A new study by researchers in Greece and the US has found that fruit flies can discriminate between normal and heavy hydrogen (deuterium) isotopes, which adds weight to a new theory of how ...

Heavy Pyridine Crystallizes Differently

Jan 07, 2009

(PhysOrg.com) -- The nuclei of ordinary hydrogen atoms contain only a single proton. If a neutron is added, the hydrogen becomes deuterium. In principle, molecules that contain deuterium in place of hydrogen ...

Recommended for you

Why plants don't get sunburn

Oct 29, 2014

Plants rely on sunlight to make their food, but they also need protection from its harmful rays, just like humans do. Recently, scientists discovered a group of molecules in plants that shields them from ...

Viral switches share a shape

Oct 27, 2014

A hinge in the RNA genome of the virus that causes hepatitis C works like a switch that can be flipped to prevent it from replicating in infected cells. Scientists have discovered that this shape is shared by several other ...

'Sticky' ends start synthetic collagen growth

Oct 27, 2014

Rice University researchers have delivered a scientific one-two punch with a pair of papers that detail how synthetic collagen fibers self-assemble via their sticky ends.

Cell membranes self-assemble

Oct 27, 2014

A self-driven reaction can assemble phospholipid membranes like those that enclose cells, a team of chemists at the University of California, San Diego, reports in Angewandte Chemie.

Emergent behavior lets bubbles 'sense' environment

Oct 27, 2014

Tiny, soapy bubbles can reorganize their membranes to let material flow in and out in response to the surrounding environment, according to new work carried out in an international collaboration by biomedical ...

User comments : 1

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

Tausch
1 / 5 (1) Feb 11, 2013
Since binding occurs in all incidences are the pathways for olfactory discrimination (not olfactory resolution) ever questioned between flies and human test subjects?

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.