This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

proofread

The relationship between deception in negotiations and negotiators' satisfaction with their deals

negotiations
Credit: Pixabay/CC0 Public Domain

Dishonesty in negotiations is tempting because it sometimes leads to better outcomes for the deceiver, but it also lessens the deceptive negotiator's satisfaction with the bargaining experience. New Vanderbilt research finds that even when dishonesty goes undetected and positively impacts financial outcomes in a negotiation, negotiators are less happy with their deals and less willing to do future business with the person they deceived.

The study, "Does Hoodwinking Others Pay? The Psychological and Relational Consequences of Undetected Negotiator Deception," published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology and co-authored by Jessica A. Kennedy, Associate Professor of Management at Vanderbilt, supports the deceiver's "guilt account," whereby being dishonest induces more guilt than happiness, reducing the subjective value gained from a negotiation. This contrasts with the "deceiver's delight account" supported in research outside the negotiation context, which holds that dishonesty promotes positive affect, like a cheater's high.

"Too often, people think lying at the table is an easier path than honesty to an outcome that makes them happy," says Kennedy. "We show that the psychological costs of lying outweigh the , and this is even more true when financial stakes are large." In other words, money doesn't make up for a loss of integrity, psychologically.

The researchers conducted four studies to test the effect of undetected dishonesty on negotiators' outcomes, satisfaction, and interest in negotiating with the counterpart again. The studies consistently found that dishonesty elicited more guilt than positive affect and reduced negotiators' subjective value. The pattern held whether the deceiver told the lie of their own volition or was instructed to do so by another person. This effect also persisted regardless of whether participants scored high or low on three aspects of moral character: moral identity internalization, empathic concern for others, and self-control.

Larger incentives didn't help participants to rationalize lying. Participants who faced large financial incentives to lie experienced even more guilt than participants with smaller incentives. Dishonesty also impacted the negotiator's relationship, even though it went undetected. Deception led negotiators to be less interested in working again with the same counterpart. While lessened over time if the participants interacted with the counterpart again, satisfaction with the deal and relationship never recovered.

While prior research has investigated the risks of detected dishonesty, these studies explore how even undetected dishonesty can negatively impact the negotiator engaging in the deception. Whereas the harm to the victim is obvious, the harm to the deceiver is less so, and this research helps to establish it. The findings suggest that the ongoing psychological and relational costs of deception may not be worth the material gain that the deceiver sometimes attains in the moment.

"Hopefully, people start to consider high ethical standards as compatible with their happiness, not an impediment to it," Kennedy says.

More information: Alex B. Van Zant et al, Does hoodwinking others pay? The psychological and relational consequences of undetected negotiator deception, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (2022). DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000410

Citation: The relationship between deception in negotiations and negotiators' satisfaction with their deals (2023, January 30) retrieved 24 June 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2023-01-relationship-deception-satisfaction.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

In negotiations, hoodwinking others has a cost, study finds

9 shares

Feedback to editors