Computer game helps scientists understand animal camouflage

April 13, 2017
Nightjar as seen by dichromat. Credit: University of Exeter

Online computer games played by more than 30,000 people have helped scientists understand animal camouflage and colour vision.

Researchers from the University of Exeter wanted to know why many animals are dichromats (having two types of colour receptor cells in their eyes) while others, like humans, are trichromats (with three types of colour cells), and how colour affects animals' ability to detect camouflaged prey.

Game players were shown photographs and had to find camouflaged nightjar birds or nests containing eggs - either in normal colour or in a version imitating the limited colours seen by the dichromatic vision of predators like mongooses.

The prevalence of red-green colour blindness in many animals (which is also the most common form of colour blindness in humans) has led scientists to believe there must be some evolutionary advantage to seeing in just two primary colours as opposed to three.

Finding camouflaged prey has often been assumed to be easier for dichromats with red-green colour blindness, as colour has been shown to interfere with animals' ability to detect camouflaged objects.

So the Exeter team were surprised to discover that trichromats found the nightjars and eggs faster than simulated dichromats.

Nightjar as seen by a trichromat (the bird is almost exactly in the center of the image, facing right). Credit: University of Exeter

However, there were large differences in the how much the dichromats' capture times were affected by different types (such as pattern and brightness) compared to trichromats.

Over the course of the egg-hunting game, the dichromats improved faster than trichromats, so that by the end of the game they performed equally well.

It therefore remains to be seen whether dichromats could eventually learn to overcome the eggs' camouflage better than trichromats.

Lead author Dr Jolyon Troscianko, of the University of Exeter's Centre for Ecology and Conservation in Cornwall, said: "This research demonstrates the power of citizen science - using help from online participants to tackle novel scientific questions.

"Our findings suggest that the role of colour perception in spotting camouflaged objects is complex, and this could help explain why colour vision with just two receptor types is so widespread in nature."

Though dichromats can only see a limited range of colours, they could be better at differentiating between light and dark and at finding hidden objects - an advantage for certain predators.

A nightjar as seen by a trichromat (left) and a dichromat (right). Credit: University of Exeter

The authors studied the camouflage of nightjars and other ground-nesting birds in Zambia, where the nests are hunted by trichromatic animals such as humans, vervet monkeys and baboons, and by dichromats such as mongooses.

Martin Stevens, Associate Professor of Sensory and Evolutionary Ecology at the University of Exeter, said: "Camouflage is probably the most common type of defence used by prey animals in nature, but the survival advantage it provides will depend on the sensory abilities of the animals that hunt them.

"We need to know more about how camouflage may be tuned to predator vision, and in turn if and when some types of vision better allow predators to detect camouflaged prey."

One of the main vision differences in nature is the ability to see colour. Some marine mammals have only one type of receptor cell, and so cannot differentiate between colours.

Most mammals are dichromats, while humans, some fish and bees are trichromats. Many birds are tetrachromats - meaning they have four types of receptor cell - and some invertebrates have even more.

The paper, entitled "Relative advantages of dichromatic and trichromatic in camouflage breaking", is published in the journal Behavioural Ecology.

The games used in the research are still available to be played online.

Explore further: Camouflage really does reduce the chances of being eaten

More information: Jolyon Troscianko et al, Relative Advantages of Dichromatic and Trichromatic Color Vision in Camouflage Breaking, Behavioral Ecology (2017). DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arw185

Related Stories

Frogs have unique ability to see color in the dark

February 28, 2017

The night vision of frogs and toads appears to be superior to that of all other animals. They have the ability to see colour even when it is so dark that humans are not able to see anything at all. This has been shown in ...

Research shows wallabies lose on the pokies

January 30, 2014

(Phys.org) —Biologists have discovered that a wallaby's perception of colour is more similar to a dog than a quokka, sparking questions as to why marsupial colour vision has evolved so selectively.

Recommended for you

Stressed-out meerkats less likely to help group

September 22, 2017

Dominant female meerkats use aggression to keep subordinates from breeding, but a new study finds this negative behavior also can result in the latter becoming less willing to help within the group.

Why poison frogs don't poison themselves

September 21, 2017

Don't let their appearance fool you: Thimble-sized, dappled in cheerful colors and squishy, poison frogs in fact harbor some of the most potent neurotoxins we know. With a new paper published in the journal Science, scientists ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.