New study finds that teaching is not essential for people to learn to make effective tools

tools
Credit: Petr Kratochvil/public domain

A new study from the University of Exeter has found that teaching is not essential for people to learn to make effective tools. The results counter established views about how human tools and technologies come to improve from generation to generation and point to an explanation for the extraordinary success of humans as a species. The study reveals that although teaching is useful, it is not essential for cultural progress because people can use reasoning and reverse engineering of existing items to work out how to make tools.

The capacity to improve the efficacy of tools and technologies from generation to generation, known as cumulative culture, is unique to humans and has driven our ecological success. It has enabled us to inhabit the coldest and most remote regions on Earth and even have a permanent base in space. The way in which our cumulative culture has boomed compared to other species however remains a mystery.

It had long been thought that the human capacity for cumulative culture was down to special methods of learning from others - such as and imitation - that enable information to be transmitted with high fidelity.

To test this idea, the researchers recreated conditions encountered during human evolution by asking groups of people to build rice baskets from everyday materials. Some people made baskets alone, while others worked were in 'transmission chain' groups, where each group member could learn from the previous person in the chain either by imitating their actions, receiving teaching or simply examining the baskets made by previous participants.

Teaching produced the most robust baskets but after six attempts all groups showed incremental improvements in the amount of rice their baskets could carry.

Dr Alex Thornton from the Centre for Ecology and Conservation at the University of Exeter's Penryn Campus in Cornwall said: "Our study helps uncover the process of incremental improvements seen in the tools that humans have used for millennia. While a knowledgeable teacher clearly brings important advantages, our study shows that this is not a limiting factor to cultural progress. Humans do much more than learn socially, we have the ability to think independently and use reason to develop new ways of doing things. This could be the secret to our success as a species."

The results of the study shed light on ancient society and help to bridge the cultural chasm between humans and other species. The researchers say that to fully understand those elements that make us different from other animals, future work should focus on the mental abilities of individuals not solely mechanisms of social learning.

Cognitive requirements of cumulative culture: teaching is useful but not essential by Elena Zwirner and Alex Thornton is published in Scientific Reports


Explore further

First demonstration of cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory

Journal information: Scientific Reports

Citation: New study finds that teaching is not essential for people to learn to make effective tools (2015, November 26) retrieved 20 October 2019 from https://phys.org/news/2015-11-essential-people-effective-tools.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.
1076 shares

Feedback to editors

User comments

Nov 26, 2015
It would seem the idea of cumulative culture adds nothing to what is already obvious and trite that how things are done can improve across generations and occasionally not depending upon lots and lots of factors which unless research councils fund time machines we cannot know.

Nov 26, 2015
It would seem the idea of cumulative culture adds nothing to what is already obvious and trite that how things are done can improve across generations


It doesn't. "Cumulative culture" is simply the name we give to the process of improvement upon improvement. That wasn't the point of the article.

The point was to examine how the culture accumulates, whether by individual discovery, by copying from others, or by teaching others.

The thing being examined is how some particular innovation, such as weaving a basket in some purposeful way, propagates through the population. The same way of making a basket can be taught, copied, or people can come up with the solution independently, and the previous hypothesis was that independent discovery or copying was not significant compared to teaching how to make baskets.

It has impact on things like historical artifacts, because say a certain type of say clay pot may not be unique to a particular culture.

Nov 30, 2015
We don't have much detail, but it seems that the improvement without teaching or imitation was by a single individual making repeated attempts. So now we need to know if that improvement will continue across generations which is the real question. Maybe the journal article deals with this process but it is vital since improvements by a single individual do not make 'culture'.

Nov 30, 2015
I guess it also depends on the complexity of the task. A rice basket is a pretty straightforward technology and many design attempts will converge on one solution.

When we go to more complicated/non-intuitive technologies then things get more interesting (e.g. building a sturdy bridge or an efficient hearth or even fashioning a sturdy piece of rope). There individually brilliant ideas can confer significant advantage when handed down the 'transmission chain' - especially if it's one of those ideas that is few and far between.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more