Crayfish brain may offer rare insight into human decision making

Jun 15, 2010
Crayfish, similar to organisms of higher complexity, observe their environment, and then make values-based decisions, the research shows. Credit: David D. Yager/Jens Herberholz, University of Maryland

Crayfish make surprisingly complex, cost-benefit calculations, finds a University of Maryland study, opening the door to a new line of research that may help unravel the cellular brain activity involved in human decisions.

The Maryland researchers conclude that make an excellent, practical model for identifying the specific and neurochemistry of . They believe their study is the first to isolate individual crayfish neurons involved in value-based decisions. Currently, there's no direct way to do this with a human brain.

The study will be published in the , and is being released online today.

"Matching individual neurons to the decision making processes in the human brain is simply impractical for now," explains University of Maryland psychologist Jens Herberholz, the study's senior author. "History has shown that findings made in the invertebrate nervous systems often translate to more complex organisms. It's unlikely to be exactly the same, but it can inform our understanding of the human brain, nonetheless. The basic organization of neurons and the underlying are similar, involving serotonin and , for example."

Herberholz adds that his lab's work may inform ongoing studies in rodents and primates. "Combining the findings from different animal models is the only practical approach to work out the complexities of human decision making at the cellular level."

Specific findings and conclusions

The experiments offered the crayfish stark decisions - a choice between finding their next meal and becoming a meal for an apparent predator. In deciding on a course of action, they carefully weighed the risk of attack against the expected reward, Herberholz says.

Using a non-invasive method that allowed the crustaceans to freely move, the researchers offered juvenile Louisiana Red Swamp crayfish a simultaneous threat and reward: ahead lay the scent of food, but also the apparent approach of a predator. In some cases, the "predator" (actually a shadow) appeared to be moving swiftly, in others slowly. To up the ante, the researchers also varied the intensity of the odor of food.

How would the animals react? Did the risk of being eaten outweigh their desire to feed? Should they "freeze" - in effect, play dead, hoping the predator would pass by, while the crayfish remained close to its meal - or move away from both the predator and food?

To make a quick escape, the crayfish would flip their tails and swim backwards - an action preceded by a strong, measurable electric neural impulse. The specially designed tanks could non-invasively pick up and record these electrical signals. This allowed the researchers to identify the activation patterns of specific neurons during the decision-making process.

Although tail-flipping is a very effective escape strategy against natural predators, it adds critical distance between a foraging animal and its next meal.

The crayfish took decisive action in a matter of milliseconds. When faced with very fast shadows, they were significantly more likely to freeze than tail-flip away. The researchers conclude that there's little incentive for retreat when the predator appears to be moving too rapidly for escape, and the crayfish would lose its own opportunity to eat. This was also true when the food odor was the strongest, raising the benefit of staying close to the expected reward. A strong predator stimulus, however, was able to override an attractive food signal, and crayfish decided to flip away under these conditions.

"Our results indicate that when the respective values of tail-flipping and freezing change, the crayfish adjust their choices accordingly, thus preserving adaptive action selection," the study concludes. "We have now shown that crayfish, similar to organisms of higher complexity, integrate different sensory stimuli that are present in their environment, and they select a behavioural output according to the current values for each choice."

The next step is to identify the specific cellular and neurochemical mechanisms involved in crayfish decisions, which is more feasible in an animal with fewer and accessible neurons, Herberholz says. That research is now underway.

Explore further: Rescued 'abandoned' penguin chicks survival similar to colony rates

Related Stories

Crayfish win by cheating

Mar 25, 2009

(PhysOrg.com) -- A study conducted at UQ's Moreton Bay Research Station has found, when it comes to crayfish, size really does matter.

Hope for Ridding Lakes of Clawed Invader

Jul 31, 2006

The rusty crayfish - a voracious, bullying exotic that has visited ecological havoc on numerous Wisconsin lakes - may have finally met its match.

Inside the brain of a crayfish

Sep 01, 2007

Voyage to the bottom of the sea, or simply look along the bottom of a clear stream and you may spy lobsters or crayfish waving their antennae. Look closer, and you will see them feeling around with their legs and flicking ...

What gives us sunburn protects crayfish against bacteria

Sep 24, 2007

The production of melanin gives us sunburns, but it also helps invertebrate animals to encapsulate attacking fungi and parasites. Uppsala University researchers, in collaboration with Korean and Thai colleagues, can now ...

Recommended for you

Secret wing colours attract female fruit flies

16 hours ago

Bright colours appear on a fruit fly's transparent wings against a dark background as a result of light refraction. Researchers from Lund University in Sweden have now demonstrated that females choose a mate ...

Pigeons and people play the odds when rewards are higher

18 hours ago

(Phys.org) —If you were weighing the risks, would you choose to receive a guaranteed $100, or take a 50/50 chance of winning either $200 or nothing? Researchers at the University of Alberta have shown that ...

User comments : 3

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

gwrede
not rated yet Jun 15, 2010
Oh, please. These scientists may go on, and then, suddenly, 50 years from now, stumble upon the earth-shattering revelation, that the common mosquito behaves exactly the same as the crayfish when set up in a similar experiment. Imagine the headlines!!

Anybody who's lived in a mosquito area knows that they weigh threat against reward. They also have moods, as in calm or stressed, the latter originating from a narrow escape from being squashed. The mosquitoes also do a passable job of short-range navigating, and of course they find food, mates, and a suitable environment for their offspring.

That is no mean feat, considering that the mosquito brain is measured in 1/100s of a millimeter, while the crayfish brain is measured in entire millimeters. Converted to volume, that means that the crayfish has one million times the volume for its brain. Really. So, the crayfish can afford to be pretty sloppy with its brain structure, while still feeling comfortable with its tasks.
gwrede
not rated yet Jun 15, 2010
You know, weighing between threat and reward was actually invented the very day the first organism learned to move.

That means that it is likely that an amoeba (a single celled organism, obviously without a brain, as we understand it) should already show some primitive parts of reward/threat calculations. (It's already been established that the amoeba is capable of Pavlovian learning without a brain, actually decades ago.)

All this points to a disturbing result: it might one day dawn upon the community that a person's Me, or Self, or My Soul, or Self-Awareness, or Person -- is actually only the interplay of merely 15 neurons in the brain. That would give clergymen, creationists, spiritists, and the like, a serious jolt.

But what I've read in the last 40 years, seems to approach asymptotically this conclusion. Now, I'm not insisting this is the case, but rather, the more I see the more I'd be surprised if anything else turns out to be the answer.
sciencegirl24
5 / 5 (1) Jun 16, 2010
I think you're missing the point. The fact that these scientists could change the behavioral output of the animal based on different internal and external features is very exciting. Even more exciting is the potential to understand this on a cellular and neurochemical level because the neurocircuitry is so well understood and easily accessible. Now this group can try and determine what neuromodulators play a role in this decision-making and just where it's taking place.