How the hawkmoth sees, hovers and tracks flowers in the dark

June 11, 2015, Georgia Institute of Technology
A hawkmoth clings to a robotic flower used to study the insect’s ability to track the moving flower under low light conditions. The research shows that the creatures can slow their brains to improve vision under low light conditions while continuing to perform demanding tasks. Credit: Rob Felt, Georgia Tech

It's difficult enough to see things in the dark, but what if you also had to hover in mid-air while tracking a flower moving in the wind? That's the challenge the hummingbird-sized hawkmoth (Manduca sexta) must overcome while feeding on the nectar of its favorite flowers.

Using high-speed infrared cameras and 3-D-printed robotic flowers, scientists have now learned how this insect juggles these complex sensing and control challenges - all while adjusting to changing light conditions. The work shows that the creatures can slow their brains to improve vision under low-light conditions - while continuing to perform demanding tasks.

What the researchers have discovered could help the next generation of small flying robots operate efficiently under a broad range of lighting conditions. The research, supported by the National Science Foundation and Air Force Office of Scientific Research, will be reported in the June 12 issue of the journal Science.

"There has been a lot of interest in understanding how animals deal with challenging sensing environments, especially when they are also doing difficult tasks like hovering in mid-air," said Simon Sponberg, an assistant professor in the School of Physics and School of Applied Physiology at the Georgia Institute of Technology. "This is also a very significant challenge for micro air vehicles."

Scientists already knew that the , which feed on flower nectar during the evening and at dusk and dawn, use specialized eye structures to maximize the amount of light they can capture. But they also surmised that the insects might be slowing their nervous systems to make the best use of this limited light. But if they were slowing their brains to see better, wouldn't that hurt their ability to hover and track the motion of flowers?

Using infrared cameras and robotic flowers, scientists have learned how the hawkmoth juggles complex sensing and control challenges to hover in mid-air and track flowers while adjusting to changing light conditions. The work shows that the creatures can slow their brains to improve vision under low-light conditions -- while continuing to perform demanding tasks. Credit: Georgia Tech Video

Sponberg and colleagues at the University of Washington studied this question using high-speed infrared cameras and nectar-dispensing robotic flowers that could be moved from side-to-side at different rates. While varying both the light conditions and the frequency at which the flowers moved, the researchers studied how well free-flying moths kept their tongues - known as proboscises - in the flowers.

They also measured real flowers blowing in the wind to determine the range of motion the insects had to contend with in the wild.

"We expected to see a trade-off with the moths doing significantly worse at tracking flowers in low light conditions," said Sponberg. "What we saw was that while the moths did slow down, that only made a difference if the flower was moving rapidly - faster than they actually move in nature."

A hawkmoth with its proboscis extended approaches a robotic flower on which another hawkmoth has already landed. The research shows that the creatures can slow their brains to improve vision under low-light conditions -- while continuing to perform demanding tasks. Credit: Credit: Rob Felt, Georgia Tech

In the experiments, the moths tracked robotic flowers that were oscillating at rates of up to 20 hertz - twenty oscillations per second. That was considerably faster than the two-hertz maximum rate observed in real flowers. Because the moth's wings beat at a rate of about 25 strokes per second, they had to adjust their direction of movement with nearly every wingstroke - a major sensing, computational and control accomplishment.

"This is really an extreme behavior, though the moth makes it look simple and elegant," said Sponberg. "To maneuver like this is really quite challenging. It's an extreme behavior from both a sensory and motor control perspective."

In the natural world, light intensity varies 10 billion-fold from noon on a sunny day to midnight a cloudy evening. Operating in that range of luminosity is a challenge for both moths and the sensors on human-engineered systems. Understanding how natural systems adjust to this range of conditions could therefore have broader benefits.

"If we want to have robots or machine vision systems that are working under this broad range of conditions, understanding how these moths function under these varying light conditions would be very useful," Sponberg said.

Georgia Tech researcher Simon Sponberg holds a hawkmoth (Manduca sexta). Research on this hummingbird-sized insect shows that the creatures can slow their brains to improve vision under low-light conditions -- while continuing to perform demanding tasks. Credit: Credit: Rob Felt, Georgia Tech

To gather the data reported in this paper, the researchers used a robotic flower able to move in one dimension. Recently, they've used the actuator devices from a 3-D printer to build a robotic flower that moves in two or three dimensions, providing an additional challenge for the moths. In future research, Sponberg and his colleagues hope to incorporate their robotic flower into a low-speed wind tunnel to study the aerodynamic challenges the moths overcome - including the role of wing vortices and the flow-effect interaction of the insect's wings with the flowers.

The hawkmoth has been studied extensively to investigate the fundamental principles governing the development and function of its neural system, noted Tom Daniel, a professor in the Department of Biology and co-director of the Institute for Neuroengineering at the University of Washington. Daniel's research group has experimentally characterized the response of flying hawkmoths, using a sensory input comprised of the linear sum of sine waves.

Sponberg's paper, for which much of the data collection was done at the University of Washington while he was a postdoctoral researcher there, extends application of the "sum of sines" approach, Daniel said.

"Simon's work took the formal methods of control theory to dissect out how neural circuits adapt to vast ranges of luminance levels," he explained. "By looking at the time delays in the movement dynamics of a freely-flying moth - interacting with the input of a robotically moved flower - Simon was able to extract the luminance dependent processing of the moth's central nervous system."

Human engineered devices must often operate at various speeds and in different environments. Seeing how well an animal with a tiny brain was able to track complicated movements and adjust its performance to different light levels was a surprising result of the work, Sponberg said.

"This was an interesting example of how an organism can tune its brain to maintain its ability to gather food," he added. "The moths do suffer a trade-off by slowing their brains, but that trade-off doesn't end up mattering because it only affects their ability to track movements that don't exist in the natural way that flowers blow in the wind."

Explore further: 3-D printing blossoms into powerful new tool for ecologists

More information: Luminance-dependent visual processing enables moth flight in low light, www.sciencemag.org/lookup/doi/ … 1126/science.aaa3042

Related Stories

3-D printing blossoms into powerful new tool for ecologists

April 14, 2015

3D printing has been used to make everything from cars to medical implants. Now, ecologists are using the technology to make artificial flowers, which they say could revolutionise our understanding of plant-pollinator interactions. ...

Moths wired two ways to take advantage of floral potluck

December 6, 2012

Moths are able to enjoy a pollinator's buffet of flowers – in spite of being among the insect world's picky eaters – because of two distinct "channels" in their brains, scientists at the University of Washington and University ...

Study: Hawkmoths use humidity to sense nectar

June 28, 2012

(Phys.org) -- People assume that a flower's scent, color and shape attract insects to settle on a flower to sip nectar and, thereby, pollinate the plant. But new research shows that a more relevant sensory apparatus may help ...

Insect mating behavior has lessons for drones

May 29, 2015

Male moths locate females by navigating along the latter's pheromone (odor) plume, often flying hundreds of meters to do so. Two strategies are involved to accomplish this: males must find the outer envelope of the pheromone ...

Recommended for you

A new, dynamic view of chromatin movements

January 18, 2018

In cells, proteins tightly package the long thread of DNA into pearl necklace-like complexes known as chromatin. Scientists at EPFL show for the first time how chromatin moves, answering longstanding questions about how its ...

How living systems compute solutions to problems

January 17, 2018

How do decisions get made in the natural world? One possibility is that the individuals or components in biological systems collectively compute solutions to challenges they face in their environments. Consider that fish ...

Scale-eating fish adopt clever parasitic methods to survive

January 17, 2018

Think of them as extra-large parasites. A small group of fishes—possibly the world's cleverest carnivorous grazers—feeds on the scales of other fish in the tropics. The different species' approach differs: some ram their ...

2 comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

NiteSkyGerl
2.3 / 5 (3) Jun 11, 2015
This is an interesting model of the behavior in the immediate environment, but one wonders how much olfactory perception has to do with the moth being in the vicinity of the flower in the first place.
NiteSkyGerl
1 / 5 (2) Jun 11, 2015
- Duplicate deleted -

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.