This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

proofread

With little knowledge comes great confidence: Study reveals relationship between knowledge and attitudes toward science

With little knowledge comes great confidence
Possible relationships between confidence and knowledge. a, Comparison of different theoretical models. Perfect metacognition predicts a linear relationship between knowledge (K) and confidence (C) (yellow line, the more one knows, the more one is confident about one’s knowledge). The Dunning–Kruger effect describes poor or no correlation between knowledge and confidence (purple dashed line), similar to what independence would predict (blue dotted line). Other possible relations include confidence increasing (linearly, dashed purple; nonlinearly, solid purple) or decreasing (solid green) with knowledge. b, Comparison of expected patterns of average normalized incorrect (I) to ‘don’t know’ (DK) ratios assuming different response models. The x axis goes from zero correct answers (all answers are either I or DK) to all correct answers (zero I and zero DK). Perfect calibration (yellow line, respondents never answer incorrectly); DK-like expectation (green dashed lines, respondents are more likely to answer incorrectly at low knowledge levels); random answering (blue dotted line, respondents are as likely to answer I as DK); expected distribution when respondents wage their bets, with incorrect answers growing slowly with K (large-dashed gray line). Credit: Nature Human Behavior (2023). DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01677-8

Overconfidence has long been recognized as a critical problem in judgment and decision making.

According to Dr. Cristina Mendonça, one of the lead authors of a new study published in Nature Human Behavior, "Overconfidence occurs when individuals subjectively assess their aptitude to be higher than their objective accuracy [and] has long been recognized as a critical problem in judgment and decision making. Past research has shown that miscalibrations in the internal representation of accuracy can have severe consequences but how to gauge these miscalibrations is far from trivial."

In the case of scientific knowledge, overconfidence might be particularly significant, as the lack of awareness of one's own ignorance can impact behaviors, pose risks to public policies, and even jeopardize health.

In the study published today, researchers examined four large surveys conducted over a span of 30 years in Europe and the U.S., and sought to develop a novel confidence metric that would be indirect, independent across scales, and applicable to diverse contexts.

The research team used surveys with the format "True," "False," "Don't know" and devised a ratio of incorrect to "Don't Know" answers as an overconfidence metric, positing that incorrect answers could indicate situations where respondents believed they knew the answer but were mistaken, thus demonstrating overconfidence. In the words of Dr. Mendonça, "This metric has the advantages of being easy to replicate and not requiring individuals to compare themselves to others nor to explicitly state how confident they are."

The results revealed two key findings. First, overconfidence tended to grow faster than knowledge, reaching its peak at intermediate levels of knowledge. Second, respondents with intermediate knowledge and high confidence also displayed the least positive attitudes towards science.

According to André Mata, one of the authors of the study, "This combination of overconfidence and negative attitudes towards science is dangerous, as it can lead to the dissemination of false information and , in both cases with great confidence."

To validate their conclusions, the researchers developed a new survey, quantitatively analyzed the work of other colleagues and used two direct, non-comparative metrics of trust, which confirmed the trend that trust increases faster than knowledge.

The implications of these findings are far-reaching and challenge conventional assumptions about strategies.

According to the study coordinator Dr. Gonçalves-Sá, "Science communication and outreach often prioritize simplifying for broader audiences. While presenting simplified information might offer a basic level of knowledge, it could also lead to increased gaps among those with some (albeit little) knowledge. There is a common sense idea that 'a little knowledge is a dangerous thing' and, at least in the case of scientific knowledge that might very well be the case."

Thus, the study suggests that efforts to promote knowledge, if not accompanied by an equivalent effort to convey a certain awareness of how much remains to be understood, can have unexpected effects. It also suggests that interventions should be targeted at individuals with intermediate knowledge, since they make up the majority of the population and tend to have the least positive attitudes towards science.

Nevertheless, the researchers caution that their confidence metric might not generalize to topics outside of scientific knowledge and surveys that penalize wrong answers heavily. The study also does not imply causality, and individual and cultural differences were observed.

Overall, this paper calls for further exploration of integrative metrics that can accurately measure both knowledge and confidence while considering potential differences in constructs.

More information: Lackner, S. et al. Intermediate levels of scientific knowledge are associated with overconfidence and negative attitudes towards science, Nature Human Behavior (2023). DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01677-8 www.nature.com/articles/s41562-023-01677-8

Journal information: Nature Human Behaviour

Provided by LIP

Citation: With little knowledge comes great confidence: Study reveals relationship between knowledge and attitudes toward science (2023, September 14) retrieved 27 April 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2023-09-knowledge-great-confidence-reveals-relationship.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Overconfidence bolsters anti-scientific views, study finds

1185 shares

Feedback to editors