When good intentions aren't supported by social science evidence

When good intentions aren't supported by social science evidence
Successful group outcomes aren’t guaranteed by the simple recipe of ‘Just add diversity.’ Credit: www.shutterstock.com

You'd be forgiven for assuming a quick and sure way to multiply profits and amplify organizational success is to increase the gender and racial diversity of any group. According to claims in the mainstream media, the effects of gender and racial diversity are universally favorable. News stories tend to mirror this 2014 Washington Post article's claim that "researchers have long found ties between having women on a company's board of directors and better financial performance."

And as Nicholas Kristoff wrote in The New York Times in 2013:

Scholarly research suggests that the best problem-solving doesn't come from a group of the best individual problem-solvers, but from a diverse team whose members complement each other. That's an argument for leadership that is varied in every way—in gender, race, economic background and ideology.

The truth is there's actually no adequate scientific basis for these newsworthy assertions. And this lack of scientific evidence to guide such statements illustrates the troubled relations of science to advocacy and policy, that I have analyzed in an article in the current Journal of Social Issues.

A chasm between research findings and advocates' claims

I began to think more deeply about these issues during my recent service as president of the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues. This organization has worked since 1936 to join social science findings to responsible advocacy and effective social policy.

This goal is laudable, but the task is supremely challenging. As I've come to realize, different camps have varying goals. Scientists aim to produce valid knowledge. Advocates work to promote their favored causes. Policymakers hope to efficiently deploy resources to attain social and economic ends. And they're all assuming their claims are supported by the same body of .

When good intentions aren't supported by social science evidence
Does who fills these empty boardroom chairs affect the bottom line? Credit: www.shutterstock.com

In politically sensitive areas, advocates may eagerly invoke social scientific data that support their objectives but ignore nonsupportive findings. They may highlight politically congenial findings that are unrepresentative of the available scientific knowledge.

Researchers, in turn, may fail to communicate their findings effectively. Communication is challenging when study outcomes are more complex and less affirming of advocates' goals than what they desire and expect.

These issues often arise when research addresses controversial questions of social inequality. That's where social science myths can and do emerge.

Case study: diversity research

To illustrate these problems, consider two prominent social science myths about diversity.

One concerns the effects of the of corporate boards of directors on firms' financial performance. The other pertains to the effects of the gender and of workgroups on their performance.

Advocates for diversity generally maintain that the addition of women to corporate boards enhances corporate financial success. And they hold that diversity in task groups enhances their effectiveness.

Abundant findings have accumulated on both of these questions – more than 140 studies of corporate boards and more than 100 studies of sociodemographic diversity in task groups. Both sets of studies have produced mixed outcomes. Some studies show positive associations of diversity to these outcomes, and some show negative associations.

Social scientists use meta-analyses to integrate such findings across the relevant studies. Meta-analyses represent all the available studies on a particular topic by quantitatively averaging their findings and also examining differences in studies' results. Cherry-picking is not allowed.

Taking into account all of the available research on corporate boards and diversity of task groups, the net effects are very close to a null, or zero, average. Also, economists' studies that carefully evaluate causal relations have typically failed to find that women cause superior corporate performance. The most valid conclusion at this point is that, on average, diversity neither helps nor harms these important outcomes.

Given these overall findings, further studies are needed to identify the conditions under which diversity has positive or negative effects. And there is some progress here.

For example, research suggests that diversity tends to make decision-making groups more effective if their members create norms that foster personal ties across the races and genders as well as the exchange of ideas. Also, a positive and inclusive mindset about diversity increases the chances of favorable effects on group performance.

But such conditions are often absent. Diversity can create tensions within groups, and the newly introduced female or minority group members may encounter resistance that makes it difficult for them to gain a foothold in decision-making. It's hardly surprising that the results of empirical studies are inconsistent. These kinds of interpersonal relationships are messy and complicated – it makes sense that upping diversity, on its own, wouldn't be a magical key to success.

A worthwhile social outcome

What's the harm in journalists announcing false generalizations about diversity if such statements help increase the number of women and minorities in important roles? After all, most people would agree that it would be an egregious violation of equal opportunity and antidiscrimination laws to exclude women and minorities from opportunities merely on the basis of their sex or race. Isn't any and all support for inclusion valuable? My answer to this question is no.

First of all, social science myths make a mockery of evidence-based advocacy and policy. In fact, an unusually large body of social science evidence has emerged in tests of the effects of diversity on corporate success and group performance. Advocacy and policy should build on this research, not ignore it.

Myths also set people up to expect that corporate financial gains and superior group performance follow easily from diversity. Of course they don't. That expectation could sideline people from understanding and overcoming diversity's challenges.

Finally, false generalizations can impede progress toward better science that may disentangle the causes of diversity's varied effects on group and organizational success.

Social scientists should freely admit that diversity science doesn't have all the answers. At the same time, they should not silently tolerate distortions of available scientific knowledge to fit advocacy goals. Ideally, researchers are honest brokers who communicate consensus scientific findings to the broader public. Only then can social science make a meaningful contribution to building sound social policy.

Social justice goals are valid on their own

Many advocates and policymakers share the admirable goal of producing a more just society. But they're narrow-minded if they focus only on whether diversity and inclusion foster outcomes such as business profits or effective group problem-solving. The more fundamental gains from diversity pertain to social justice. Diversity and inclusion can serve social justice goals by countering discrimination that may have put women and minorities at a disadvantage.

Beyond countering possible discrimination lies an even more fundamental consideration – that of equitable representation. This principle holds that citizens in democracies should have equal access to influencing the decisions that shape their lives. To the extent that women and minorities are not represented in decision-making groups in proportion to their numbers in the population, they are unlikely to have their interests fairly represented.

As political scientists have pointed out, the ideals of democracy are violated if decision-making is dominated by the rich, the white and the male. Then the needs of the poor, the minorities and the female likely are neglected.

Most advocates, policymakers and may not be aware of sharp divergence in their claims about diversity. Yet, policy based on sound should be a shared goal. Without understanding the causal relations in society that this research helps identify, policymakers lower the odds they'll reach their targets. Policy based on myths and hunches has little chance of success. To achieve evidence-based policy, all parties should take a close look at what diversity research has produced so far. Rather than selectively featuring congenial results, they should work together to untangle 's complex effects on group and organizational performance.


Explore further

Study finds greater productivity and earnings in family-friendly organisations with gender diversity

This article was originally published on The Conversation. Read the original article.
The Conversation

Citation: When good intentions aren't supported by social science evidence (2016, March 10) retrieved 18 October 2019 from https://phys.org/news/2016-03-good-intentions-social-science-evidence.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.
12 shares

Feedback to editors

User comments

Mar 10, 2016
"As political scientists have pointed out, the ideals of democracy are violated if decision-making is dominated by the rich, the white and the male. Then the needs of the poor, the minorities and the female likely are neglected."
A typical statement for which there is no evidence whatsoever.

Mar 10, 2016
This comment has been removed by a moderator.

Mar 10, 2016
To the extent that women and minorities are not represented in decision-making groups in proportion to their numbers in the population, they are unlikely to have their interests fairly represented.


Equality in democratic representation is not about special interests that go against the common interest. Not women vs. men, or race vs. race. If that were the case, democracy would be a tyranny of the majority since the majority would always get their way at the expense of the rest. Democracy is about mutual interest.

Therefore:

To the extent that the interests of these groups are not common to all people, they shoud not get representation.

To the extent that the interest of these groups are in the common interests of all people, they can be represented by anyone - not necessarily a member of their own group - because they share the same interests.

So the argument that women and minorities have to be proportionally present in decisionmaking bodies is false.

Mar 10, 2016
Of course in practice, democracy is riddled with special interest groups that try to cut a bigger slice of the cake for themselves, and that is part of the problem because everyone is pulling in opposite directions and the whole process becomes paralyzed.

So if we take the view that women and men have opposing interests, and that it's this opposition that matters for justice, then we'll never get any resolution on anything because half the population is always going to deny whatever the other half is attempting to do, claiming that the gain of one is the loss of the other. Or in the case of minorities, even if you have proportional representation they'll still be minorities and still get overrun by the majority, and that wouldn't be justice.

That is the problem of "class warfare", or dividing people into arbitrary groups and then pitching them against each other. The "social justice" movement simply translates the problem from socioeconomics to ethnicity and gender.

Mar 10, 2016
This comment has been removed by a moderator.

Mar 11, 2016
life is most important in life is true
@Dave
this can't be true as life is defined by nonexistence and death, therefore, life is irrelevant as it's primary definition comes from it's termination, therefore DEATH must be far more important in life as it is what drives and motivates living creatures to succeed or survive

more importantly, as we are all a mass of organic chemistry, life must also be emergent from something, therefore it is more a side effect than a specific anything
(it is also not clearly defined and therefore can't decisively be claimed to be more important to anything)
Now, considering life is made from dead and or non-living organic parts, how does that qualify life to be important?
it doesn't, because only death can be more important- after all, it is a strong motivator to survive

so, quit repeatedly regurgitating your bs mantra in science threads as it is meaningless and YOU can't even define it [the mantra] or define what life is

Mar 12, 2016
To the extent that the interests of these groups are not common to all people, they shoud not get representation.

Are you thinking of interests that should be common to all people, by whatever criterion, or interests that actually are common to all? If the latter, I have a hard time thinking of any. I would have thought clean air and water are common interests, but not everyone agrees. The rights to life, liberty and the pursuit of happiness promised in the US constitution look like good candidates for interests common to all, but even the people who wrote the constitution denied those rights to slaves and to the original inhabitants of the country. If you propose an interest X common to all, are you sure that nobody will say "but those people over there don't deserve X"? So how do you find universal agreement on anything specific?

Mar 13, 2016
As every decent researcher is aware it would be an accidental coincidence if politically correct equals scientifically correct.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more