Study Shows How We Evolved Different Personalities

Nov 13, 2008 By Lisa Zyga feature
The researchers performed simulations of the above game to understand how behavior variation evolves in a population. Player 1 (P1) chooses to trust or not trust Player 2 (P2). If trusted, P2 chooses to take advantage of P1 to gain a higher pay-off, or be trustworthy. The pay-off relation is 0 < s < r < 1. P1’s cost of sampling P2 is c, where 0 < c < s. Image credit: J. M. McNamara et al. ©2008 The Royal Society.

(PhysOrg.com) -- Although members of the same species share more than 99 percent of their genetic makeup, individuals often have small differences, such as in their appearance, susceptibility to disease, and life expectancy. Another difference, one that has gone overlooked from the evolutionary perspective, is personality variation. Even identical twins can have personality types at opposite ends of the spectrum.

This observation has led researchers to ask how evolution may have selected for personality variation within a species. A team from the UK has recently suggested a novel yet simple answer: that variation begets variation. They explain how there is no single ideal personality (as there is an ideal hand or eye, which we all share), but nature instead promotes different personalities.

In their recent study, John McNamara, Philip Stephens, and Alasdair Houston from the University of Bristol, and Sasha Dall of the University of Exeter, Cornwall Campus, explain how natural selection can prevent individuals in a species from evolving toward a single optimum personality, using a game theory scenario.

In their study, the researchers focus on the evolution of trust and trustworthiness. The game scenario they use is a variant of the Prisoner’s Dilemma. First, Player 1 chooses to trust or not trust Player 2. Not trusting gives Player 1 a small payoff, and Player 2 gets nothing. If Player 1 trusts Player 2, and Player 2 is trustworthy, then both players receive the same medium-size payoff. But if Player 1 trusts Player 2, and Player 2 isn’t trustworthy, Player 1 receives nothing, and Player 2 receives the maximum pay-off. In other words, Player 1 takes a risk if choosing to trust Player 2.

At this point, it seems that Player 2 should always choose to be untrustworthy, so that he always receives the maximum payoff. However, as in real life, the game is iterative. And – this is the important factor – Player 1 can do some background research on Player 2, and find out how often Player 2 has been trustworthy in the past. If Player 2 has a record of being untrustworthy, then Player 1 probably won’t trust him.

This “social awareness” comes at a cost for Player 1, so Player 1 must decide if the cost is worth the information. If a population of Player 2’s has variation in its records of trustworthiness, then Player 1 could learn useful information by learning a Player 2’s history. (Realistic methods of acquiring information include, for example, talking to third parties or observing facial expression.) But if a Player 2 population generally has the same records, then the cost of social awareness wouldn’t be worthwhile for Player 1.

In simulations with multiple players, individual patterns of trust and trustworthiness were allowed to evolve freely. By watching simulations of the game, the researchers found that the Player 2 population evolved variability in trustworthiness in response to sampling by the Player 1 population. For the Player 2 population, variation was the best strategy for gaining the trust of Player 1, and then exploiting that trust to maximize their pay-off on occasion. This variation, in turn, meant that Player 1 could gain helpful information by paying the cost of being socially aware – which, once again, provoked more variation in the Player 2 population.

The researchers noted several interesting results of the game. If the Player 1 population was too trusting, the Player 2 population exploited that, and became less trustworthy. Dall said the team was pleasantly surprised by two results: that the model predicted behavioral variation in both player types, and also predicted two distinct variation patterns for Player 2’s behavior.

“Not only were we able to explain why variation should be maintained as social interactions become more extensive, we were able to explain how discrete behavioral types might evolve in otherwise continuous behavioral traits,” Dall said to PhysOrg.com.

As he elaborated, the presence of a few socially aware Player 1’s will not only keep the Player 2’s in check, but also allow for more variation among Player 1’s.

“You only need a certain number of samplers to enforce trustworthy Player 2 behavior, and so there will be a limit to the numbers of samplers that will be maintained by selection. Once samplers are common enough, everyone else should adopt unconditional, cost-free Player 1 behavior.” In other words, some Player 1’s will always trust, while other Player 1’s will never trust one another.

As the researchers concluded, even though this study focuses on a specific model, the general finding that variation begets variation in social contexts has broad implications for understanding evolution and game theory. Past results in game theory have discovered individual differences in trust and trustworthiness, and now studies like this one help to explain this variation. This study and others also show that evolutionary game theorists cannot ignore the importance of individual variation in their models. Meanwhile, the researchers will continue to investigate exactly why we have different personalities.

“More generally, the question of ‘why personality variation evolves’ requires a more complex answer, which we're only just starting to unravel as evolutionary biologists,” Dall said. “The chances are that there isn't just one reason, and which particular reason is relevant depends on the context. So far, our social awareness reason is one of the few that has been proposed to explain variation in a cooperative context. Social awareness also appears to work in an aggressive context: individuals adopt consistent levels of aggression to avoid getting in real fights, since if someone can predict you're going to be aggressive, they will avoid provoking you; individual differences arise via frequency dependence again, as the more aggression there is around you, the less you should bother fighting – this is the famous Hawk-Dove game outcome.”

More information: McNamara, John M.; Stephens, Philip A.; Dall, Sasha R. X.; Houston, Alasdair I. “Evolution of trust and trustworthiness: social awareness favours personality differences.” Proceedings of the Royal Society, doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1182.

Copyright 2008 PhysOrg.com.
All rights reserved. This material may not be published, broadcast, rewritten or redistributed in whole or part without the express written permission of PhysOrg.com.

Explore further: Best of Last Week – Evidence of quark-gluon interactions, new portable device hack and why we may never live forever

add to favorites email to friend print save as pdf

Related Stories

Moto X born of new attitudes at Motorola Mobility

Sep 26, 2013

Paul Pierce remembers the reaction his team of designers elicited from their engineering colleagues when they proposed a smartphone with a gently curved back that would nestle into a person's hand.

Playing games shows how personalities evolved

Oct 29, 2008

(PhysOrg.com) -- Why do some people co-operate while others are very selfish? Research by the universities of Bristol and Exeter offers a new explanation as to why such a wide range of personality traits has ...

Deal or no deal? The role of emotions in negotiating offers

Oct 15, 2008

Do skilled negotiators simply go with their gut instinct every time or are they just extremely calculating, figuring out all possible outcomes before making a choice? Columbia University researchers examined how emotions ...

15 human genomes each week

Jul 02, 2008

The Wellcome Trust Sanger Institute has sequenced the equivalent of 300 human genomes in just over six months. The Institute has just reached the staggering total of 1,000,000,000,000 letters of genetic code that will be ...

Recommended for you

Computer games give a boost to English

7 hours ago

If you want to make a mark in the world of computer games you had better have a good English vocabulary. It has now also been scientifically proven that someone who is good at computer games has a larger ...

Saddam Hussein—a sincere dictator?

12 hours ago

Are political speeches manipulative and strategic? They could be – when politicians say one thing in public, and privately believe something else, political scientists say. Saddam Hussein's legacy of recording private discussions ...

Oldest representative of a weird arthropod group

12 hours ago

Biologists at Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitaet (LMU) in Munich have assigned a number of 435-million-year-old fossils to a new genus of predatory arthropods. These animals lived in shallow marine habitats ...

User comments : 13

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

raron
4.4 / 5 (5) Nov 13, 2008
I'm not sure if I should trust this.
zbarlici
2.2 / 5 (6) Nov 13, 2008
don`t get me wrong, i support fringe science research, but this is just plain stupid... there are way too many factors tied into the development of personalities... and we all know this! Money spent here could have done humanity a lot more good if it was spent on Polywell Development.
Modernmystic
4.4 / 5 (7) Nov 13, 2008
Well I was hoping to have some kind of discussion on this article, but velvet is here so that's impossible now...oh well.

earls
5 / 5 (5) Nov 13, 2008
Yeah, I saw "32 comments" and was impressed considering this is the umpteenth article on the Prisoner's Dilemma.

Now... Not so much.
velvetpink
1 / 5 (10) Nov 13, 2008
Hey so you all researching cause you like discussions?Thats funny also.That people are paid for discussions.Something like politicians?Yea that must be it.Arent they paying you anyway?
velvetpink
1 / 5 (9) Nov 13, 2008
Someone say ByeBye to the universal theory and ill leave!I promise!
GIR
5 / 5 (4) Nov 13, 2008
ByeBye to the universal theory
NOM
4.3 / 5 (6) Nov 13, 2008
Someone say ByeBye to the universal theory and ill leave!I promise!
ByeBye to the universal theory.

Now sod off!
velvetpink
4.2 / 5 (10) Nov 13, 2008
ByeBye to the universal theory


May you all have a great life BYEBYE!!
D666
4 / 5 (1) Nov 14, 2008
instead of wiping our ass with the money... That is, literally, what they have done here.


Do you actually know what the word "literally" actually means? If so, and you are correct, do you have a picture of them performing this act? Eye-witness testimony? I'm just curious.
zbarlici
not rated yet Nov 14, 2008
i MEANT to be graphical :)
D666
5 / 5 (1) Nov 14, 2008
i MEANT to be graphical :)


Then you shouldn't have used the word "literally". It's not an exclamation point or a superlative. It means "the following happened exactly as described. This is not a metaphor or an exaggeration or poetic license". In this case, someone would have had to actually take a handful of (hopefully paper) money, and actually drag it across their butt. Did that happen?

If you're wondering, yes this is one of my pet peeves. I once read a newspaper article where someone claimed to have "literally gone through hell". I thought that was great because it would have finally resolved the debate about god. Unfortunately, they were misusing the word too.

On a lighter note, another reporter once quoted Queen Elizabeth as having referred to an "anus horribilus". Perhaps she wiped her butt with it...
Level99
4 / 5 (2) Nov 14, 2008
This is somewhat typical of the mickey mouse research that goes on in many fields these days. Such is the life of an academic or researcher, if they want to remain in employment, then they have to publish.

Back in the old days, at my uni (better not mention the name as it was a national university) the psych dept funds were used on more practical research. Around every three months the head of the dept. used to buy 5 kilos of hash and a few hundred tabs of acid. I, like most students there, were not really associated with the psych dept but as intrepid students determined to further the progress of modern science we unselfishly participated (of course I never actually swallowed the tabs).

We also offered our own input to this research, like "Don't buy that crap get Afghani, and make sure you get orange barrels next time."

As for the research, it involved not only the dept. lecturers and head, assorted students, and a lot of people we had never actually seen before, but also any visiting politician who came to give a speech and actually drank the water provided for their use (probably around 600 mics per glass, give or take a thousand, as we usually had to prepare such experiments in haste).

Often, the speechmakers did not participate in our research, while at other times they drank the water and within a relatively short time appeared to have difficulty in speaking at all.

What became of all this research? Like all of the others who participated, I don't actually remember. Eventually, the funds dried up and we all had to get back to studying. I think that we took our finals a couple of years later than usual but it all worked out well in the end.