June 3, 2015

This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

Research pair outline status of decorating behavior in non-humans

Sponge decorator crabs, Hyastenus elatus, grappling at Port Stephens, New South Wales. Credit: Wikipedia.
× close
Sponge decorator crabs, Hyastenus elatus, grappling at Port Stephens, New South Wales. Credit: Wikipedia.

(Phys.org)—A pair of researchers has undertaken a study of the current status of investigation into the decorating behavior of non-human organisms. Graeme Ruxton, with the University of St. Andrews and Martin Stevens with the University of Exeter, both in the U.K. have together published a paper in Royal Society Biology Letters, describing their findings.

Human beings adorn themselves with all manner of decorations, from clothes, to jewelry to tattoos and piercings and a lot of research has been done to better understand why we do this, but other animals decorate themselves as well, as the note, but very little research has been done to better understand their motivations. In this new effort, Ruxton and Stevens conducted a search of the literature to find studies that have been done on the topic and have used what they have found to offer some theories.

The researchers note that decorator crabs appear to have garnered the most attention from researchers—they have actually evolved in a way that helps, growing little hooks on their bodies for assistance with attaching bits of coral and other material. They note also that the consensus among prior researchers is that the crabs decorate themselves to ward off predators, but thus far it has not been proven—they suggest further research be undertaken to make a stronger case. They also note that some studies have been done regarding other aquatic species, such as with other types of crabs and . There too it appears that the primary reason for decoration in most species is self preservation.

The researchers note that self decoration is not limited to , though it is less prevalent with terrestrial species, presumably because it exerts more of a toll—carrying around ornamentation can be hard work when not living in the near weightlessness of the sea. Most often, they note, it occurs with the young, shortly after birth or hatching, when it can help offspring hide. More specifically, they note that it occurs with some types of larvae carrying shields of sorts, sometimes made of fecal matter and with nymphs of assassin bugs, which use the dead carcasses of victims as backpacks to cover themselves. They note also that some species of spider use sand as camouflage and some birds use coloration they add themselves for a similar purpose, and some large ungulates use mud.

The researchers summarize the results of their efforts by nothing that non-human decoration is quite diverse and not enough is known about why it occurs. They suggest more study be undertaken by others, to learn more.

More information: The evolutionary ecology of decorating behaviour, Biology Letters, DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0325

Abstract
Many animals decorate themselves through the accumulation of environmental material on their exterior. Decoration has been studied across a range of different taxa, but there are substantial limits to current understanding. Decoration in non-humans appears to function predominantly in defence against predators and parasites, although an adaptive function is often assumed rather than comprehensively demonstrated. It seems predominantly an aquatic phenomenon—presumably because buoyancy helps reduce energetic costs associated with carrying the decorative material. In terrestrial examples, decorating is relatively common in the larval stages of insects. Insects are small and thus able to generate the power to carry a greater mass of material relative to their own body weight. In adult forms, the need to be lightweight for flight probably rules out decoration. We emphasize that both benefits and costs to decoration are rarely quantified, and that costs should include those associated with collecting as well as carrying the material.

Journal information: Biology Letters

Load comments (0)