Brief diversions may help employees improve work, study says

February 17, 2011 By Blythe Bernhard

A University of Illinois professor says people don't need to feel guilty about checking personal e-mail, chatting with co-workers or addressing other minor distractions throughout the work day.

Brief diversions may actually help people concentrate and improve their performance on more important tasks, says Alejandro Lleras, who wrote a study on the topic for the journal Cognition. Lleras works at the university's Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology in Urbana.

Lleras' research seems to contradict long-standing theories that is a finite resource that runs out after a lengthy period of focus.

Attention is more like a gas tank that refills during short breaks from the task at hand, according to Lleras' study.

A loss of attention is not the problem.

"When you are distracted, it doesn't mean you aren't to anything," Lleras said.

Priests who meditate for hours don't concentrate solely on breathing or one object, he said.

"It's not that they don't get distracted, but that they're very good at dealing with the distractions and releasing themselves from them," Lleras said. "Good meditators will get distracted and get back to their main focus very quickly."

Lleras based his theory on the idea that our senses become used to stimulus. Take a room that smells strongly of coffee. If you stay in the room all day, the scent goes away because the brain is trained to respond to differences and not constants in the environment.

Similarly, if you stare at one penny and place another coin 10 inches away, the penny in your will eventually disappear. If you blink or move, the second penny reappears because the change has jolted the brain.

The same can be true of the thought process. Sustained attention to a thought can cause that thought to disappear. But if you are given something else to think about, the original thought will seem fresh when you return to it.

To prove his theory, Lleras had 84 students focus on various numbers flashing on a computer for an hour. One group received no breaks or distractions. Other groups were told to memorize numbers and wait for those numbers to pop up on the screen. The groups that received diversions, in the form of their memorized numbers popping up, sustained their concentration.

Other groups saw their attention spans wane after 20 minutes.

"It's unrealistic to expect people to focus at high levels for a long period of time," Lleras said. "It's important to create an environment where it's OK to take small breaks."

Explore further: Brief diversions vastly improve focus, researchers find

0 shares

Related Stories

Brief diversions vastly improve focus, researchers find

February 8, 2011

A new study in the journal Cognition overturns a decades-old theory about the nature of attention and demonstrates that even brief diversions from a task can dramatically improve one's ability to focus on that task for prolonged ...

Body movements can influence problem solving (w/Videos)

May 12, 2009

Swinging their arms helped participants in a new study solve a problem whose solution involved swinging strings, researchers report, demonstrating that the brain can use bodily cues to help understand and solve complex problems.

Recommended for you

Study reveals patterns in STEM grades of girls versus boys

September 25, 2018

A new study, led by UNSW Sydney Ph.D. student Rose O'Dea, has explored patterns in academic grades of 1.6 million students, showing that girls and boys perform very similarly in STEM—including at the top of the class.

Chinese Cretaceous fossil highlights avian evolution

September 24, 2018

A newly identified extinct bird species from a 127 million-year-old fossil deposit in northeastern China provides new information about avian development during the early evolution of flight.

The first predators and their self-repairing teeth

September 24, 2018

The earliest predators appeared on Earth 480 million years ago—and they even had teeth capable of repairing themselves. A team of palaeontologists led by Bryan Shirley and Madleen Grohganz from the Chair for Palaeoenviromental ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.