Tiny worms make complex decisions, too

Tiny worms make complex decisions, too
C. elegans worm (right) escaping the predatory P. pacificus worm (left). Credit: Salk Institute

How does an animal make decisions? Scientists have spent decades trying to answer this question by focusing on the cells and connections of the brain that might be involved. Salk scientists are taking a different approach—analyzing behavior, not neurons. They were surprised to find that worms can take multiple factors into account and choose between two different actions, despite having only 302 neurons compared to approximately 86 billion in humans.

The findings, published in Current Biology on March 7, 2022, have important implications for the way researchers assess motivation and cognitive abilities in animals. What's more, the study demonstrates that complex decision-making capabilities could be encoded in small biological and artificial networks.

"Our study shows you can use a such as the worm to study something complex, like goal-directed decision-making. We also demonstrated that behavior can tell us a lot about how the brain works," says senior author Sreekanth Chalasani, associate professor in Salk's Molecular Neurobiology Laboratory. "Even simple systems like worms have different strategies, and they can choose between those strategies, deciding which one works well for them in a given situation. That provides a framework for understanding how these decisions are made in more complex systems, such as humans."

Whether eating prey or defending its , the predatory worm Pristionchus pacificus relies on biting. The team's challenge was to determine the worm's intentions when it bites.

The researchers found that P. pacificus chooses between two foraging strategies for biting its prey and competitor, another worm called Caenorhabditis elegans: 1) predatory strategy, in which its goal for biting is to kill prey, or 2) territorial strategy, in which biting is instead used to force C. elegans away from a food source. P. pacificus chooses the predatory strategy against larval C. elegans, which is easy to kill. In contrast, P. pacificus selects the territorial strategy against adult C. elegans, which is difficult to kill and outcompetes P. pacificus for food.

To the team, it appeared that P. pacificus weighed the costs and benefits of multiple potential outcomes of an action—behavior that's familiar in vertebrates but unexpected in a worm.

"Scientists have always assumed that were simple—when P. pacificus bites we thought that was always for a singular predatory purpose," says first author Kathleen Quach, a postdoctoral fellow in Chalasani's lab. "Actually, P. pacificus is versatile and can use the same action, biting C. elegans, to achieve different long-term goals. I was surprised to find that P. pacificus could leverage what seemed like failed predation into successful and goal-directed territoriality."

In the future, the scientists would like to determine which of P. pacificus' cost-benefit calculations are hard-wired or flexible. They hope more research like this will help further uncover the molecular underpinnings of decision-making.

More information: Sreekanth H. Chalasani, Flexible reprogramming of Pristionchus pacificus motivation for attacking Caenorhabditis elegans in predator-prey competition, Current Biology (2022). DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.02.033. www.cell.com/current-biology/f … 0960-9822(22)00255-X

Journal information: Current Biology

Provided by Salk Institute

Citation: Tiny worms make complex decisions, too (2022, March 7) retrieved 19 April 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2022-03-tiny-worms-complex-decisions.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Decoding the chemistry of fear

44 shares

Feedback to editors