Flies like yellow, bees like blue: How flower colors cater to the taste of pollinating insects

Flies like yellow, bees like blue: how flower colours cater to the taste of pollinating insects
Specialed flower visiting flies: a hoverfly (Eristalis tenax) (left panel), and a bee-fly (Poecilanthrax apache) (right panel). Credit: Michael Becker, Pdeley

We all know the birds and the bees are important for pollination, and we often notice them in gardens and parks. But what about flies?

Flies are the second most common type of pollinator, so perhaps we should all be taught about the bees, the flies and then the birds. While we know animals may see color differently, little was known about how fly pollination shapes the types of flowers we can find in nature.

In our new study we address this gap in our knowledge by evaluating how important fly pollinators sense and use color, and how fly pollinated flowers have evolved color signals.

The way we see influences what we choose

We know that different humans often have preferences for certain colors, and in a similar way bees prefer blue hues.

Our colleague Lea Hannah has observed that hoverflies (Eristalis tenax) are much better at distinguishing between different shades of yellow than between different blues. Other research has also reported hoverflies have innate responses to yellow colors.

Many flowering plants depend on attracting pollinators to reproduce, so the appearance of their flowers has evolved to cater to the preferences of the pollinators. We wanted to find out what this might mean for how different insects like bees or flies shape flower colors in a complex natural environment where both types of insect are present.

The Australian case study

Australia is a natural laboratory for understanding flower evolution due to its geological isolation. On the mainland Australian continent, flowers have predominately evolved colors to suit animal pollination.

Around Australia there are with different pollinators. For example, Macquarie Island has no bees, and flies are the only animal pollinator.

We assembled data from different locations, including a in mainland Australia where both bees and flies forage, to model how different insects influence flower color signal evolution.

Flies like yellow, bees like blue: how flower colours cater to the taste of pollinating insects
Typical flowers preferred by bees (Lobelia rhombifolia, left panel) and flies (Pterostylis melagramma, right panel) encountered in our study sites. Inserts show the spectral profile for each species as measured by a spectrometer. Credit: Mani Shrestha

Measuring flower colors

Since we know different animals sense color in different ways, we recorded the spectrum of different wavelengths of light reflected from the flowers with a spectrometer. We subsequently modeled these spectral signatures of plant flowers considering animal perception, allowing us to objectively quantify how signals have evolved. These analyses included mapping the evolutionary ancestry of the plants.

Generalization or specialization?

According to one school of thought, flower evolution is driven by competition between flowering plants. In this scenario, different species might have very different colors from one another, to increase their chances of being reliably identified and pollinated. This is a bit like how exclusive brands seek customers by having readily identifiable branding.

An alternative hypothesis to competition is facilitation. Plants may share preferred color signals to attract a higher number of specific insects. This explanation is like how some competing businesses can do better by being physically close together to attract many customers.

Our results demonstrate how flower color signaling has dynamically evolved depending on the availability of insect pollinators, as happens in marketplaces.

In Victoria, flowers have converged to evolve color signals preferred by their pollinators. The flowers of fly-pollinated orchids are typically yellowish-green, while closely related orchids pollinated by bees have more bluish and purple colors. The flowers appeared to share the preferred colors of their main pollinator, consistent with a facilitation hypothesis.

Our research showed flies can see differences between flowers of different species in response to the pollinator local "market."

On Macquarie Island, where flies are the only pollinators, flower colors diverge from each other—but still stay within the range of the flies' preferred colors. This is consistent with a competition strategy, where differences between plant species allow flies to more easily identify the color of recently visited flowers.

When both fly and bee pollinators are present, pollinated by flies appear to "filter out" bees to reduce the number of ineffective and opportunistic visitors. For example, in the Himalayas specialized require flies with long tongues to access floral rewards. This is similar to when a store wants to exclusively attract customers specifically interested in their product range.

Our findings on fly color vision, along with novel precision agriculture techniques, can help using as alternative pollinators of crops. It also allows us to understand that if we want to see a full range of pollinating insects including beautiful hoverflies in our parks and gardens, we need to plant a range of flower types and colors.


Explore further

Striking a balance: Trade-offs shape flower diversity

More information: Jair E. Garcia et al, Fly pollination drives convergence of flower coloration, New Phytologist (2021). DOI: 10.1111/nph.17696
Journal information: New Phytologist

Provided by The Conversation

This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.The Conversation

Citation: Flies like yellow, bees like blue: How flower colors cater to the taste of pollinating insects (2021, September 2) retrieved 21 October 2021 from https://phys.org/news/2021-09-flies-yellow-bees-blue-cater.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.
11 shares

Feedback to editors