To sting or not to sting? How bees organize defense behaviors

To sting or not to sting?
Alerted honey bee (Apis mellifera). Credit: Morgane Nouvian

When do bees sting and how do they organize their collective defense behavior against predators? An interdisciplinary team of researchers from the Universities of Constance and Innsbruck has provided new insights into these questions. Their study, published in BMC Biology, combined behavioral experiments with an innovative theoretical modeling approach based on projective simulation. It shows that individual bees decide whether to sting—or not—based on the presence and concentration of an alarm pheromone. The scientists suggest that each bee has a likelihood of stinging that is not constant, but shows at least two internal thresholds for the concentration of the pheromone: one to start stinging and one to stop stinging. The computational modeling also revealed how several environmental factors, such as the rate of predator attacks and predator diversity, likely drove the evolution of the honeybees' pheromone-based communication in their defensive behavior.

High concentrations of alarm pheromone as a stop signal

When a is attacked by a or seriously disturbed by a human who—accidentally or intentionally—got too close to the hive, the bees of the launch a coordinated counterattack to defend the colony and to scare off the trespasser. An important stimulus for them to start chasing and stinging the intruder is the presence of an alarm pheromone, which the bees carry on their stinger. In the event of an attack, the pheromone is dispersed either actively—by guard bees—or automatically upon stinging—by recruited soldiers. Thus, it carries information not only about the presence of an attacker, but also about the extent of the colony's counterattack. "The more bees have stung the intruder, the more alarm pheromone has been released with each sting and the higher its local concentration," clarifies Dr. Morgane Nouvian, a biologist from Konstanz and joint-lead author of the study together with Andrea López-Incera from Innsbruck.

To understand how individual bees from the hive may use this information to make the ultimate decision to sting for the good of the colony and possibly die as a result, the scientists observed individual stinging responses of Western Honeybees (Apis mellifera) from three colonies. Using different concentrations of natural and synthetic alarm pheromones and a dummy predator, they revealed that the aggressiveness towards the dummy—measured as the stinging likelihood—initially increases with the concentration of the alarm pheromones until it reaches a peak. However, at high concentrations, the aggressiveness drops back to a low level.

This is the first time decreasing aggressiveness at high pheromone concentrations has been demonstrated under controlled experimental conditions. "One possible function of this 'stopping' effect of high concentrations of the alarm pheromone could be to avoid over-stinging and unnecessary sacrifice when attacking an already defeated intruder," Nouvian suggests.

The "superorganism" as an evolutionary unit

In , be they honeybees or other social species such as army ants, individuals often coordinate their actions for the benefit and survival of the colony. For this reason, evolutionary selection processes in these insects acts on the group rather than the individual level. "Normally, if an organism dies, it cannot pass on its genes to the next generation anymore. In a bee colony, however, it is the queen that is responsible for reproduction. If another bee dies defending the hive but saves the queen in the process, the colony will continue to reproduce," Nouvian exemplifies. Because the bee colony functions as a single "superorganism," the behaviors of the belonging individuals can only be understood through the collective outcome to which they contribute.

To further analyze their experimental results and address this peculiarity of the evolution of collective behaviors, the scientists used computational modeling based on so-called Projective Simulation, an approach originally developed by co-author Professor Hans Briegel and his colleagues from Innsbruck. In their agent-based model, each agent, or bee, has a very limited set of percepts—the concentration of the alarm pheromone and a signal that the predator is leaving—and actions—to sting or not to sting—relevant to the defense behavior. "Our idea was to build a model that is realistic, yet minimal," explains Professor Thomas Müller, professor of philosophy at the University of Konstanz and co-author of the study. He continues: "We simulated a collective of these agents, which were called upon one after the other to perceive the current level of alarm pheromone at any given time. When a bee stings in response, the pheromone's concentration increases and the next bee's response depends on this new pheromone level."

A second important aspect of the model is that it includes a learning component: Neither the responses of individual bees nor the rules of interaction between them are predetermined. Instead, they "evolve" over many cycles of the simulation or, in other words, over many generations of the collective. "If the decisions of the individual agents are beneficial to the collective under certain environmental pressures, they are positively reinforced. This increases the likelihood that the of agents will act similarly under identical conditions," Müller says. Taken together, the agent-based approach with reinforcement learning at the group-level allowed modeling of the observed defensive behavior of honeybees from the perspective of both, the individual bees and the collective.

Putting the model to the test

Using the model and different parameter combinations, predictions could be made about the possible influence of environmental pressures on the defensive behavior of bees. For example, the simulations suggest that colonies adapt to the strongest predator they encounter. This means that colonies that primarily encounter weak predators, such as mice or toads, are less likely to sting at high concentrations than colonies that more frequently encounter strong and difficult-to-deter predators, such as bears. "For the survival of the colony, it makes perfect sense to be able to cope with the worst predator around, even if that means over-stinging some of the weaker predators," Nouvian says

The scientists also applied their model to the case of the notoriously aggressive "African bee," a subspecies of the Western honeybee. It has previously been suggested that the highly aggressive behavior of this subspecies evolved in response to higher predation rates in the tropics and to highly specialized, hard-to-deter predators, such as honey badgers. Indeed, the simulation predicted that bee populations suffering from a high predation rate and predators that take a high number of stings before stopping their attack develop stronger defense responses than those that do not.

What's next?

"We were quite happy to see that our model supports the current hypotheses on how the higher aggressiveness of African bees might have evolved. One of the next steps will be to collect empirical data from real bees in Africa to verify the results," Nouvian says.

Another step for the future is to model a more diverse population of bees. As mentioned before, there are at least two different types of bees involved in the defense attack of a real hive: guards and recruits. "In the current model, each bee in the collective followed the same decision-making process. Training a model with two different types of agents and comparing it with experimental data will be very interesting," Müller adds. In general, the modeling approach is highly versatile and can be applied to other tasks and species, providing a valuable new tool for studying the evolution of collective behavior.

More information: Andrea López-Incera et al, Honeybee communication during collective defense is shaped by predation, BMC Biology (2021). DOI: 10.1186/s12915-021-01028-x

Journal information: BMC Biology

Citation: To sting or not to sting? How bees organize defense behaviors (2021, May 26) retrieved 23 April 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2021-05-bees-defense-behaviors.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

It's bee season. To avoid getting stung, just stay calm and don't swat

17 shares

Feedback to editors