The study was done with 10 female greater mouse-eared bats (Myotis myotis) from the Orlova Chuka cave in Bulgaria. These have not been tagged yet. Credit: Stefan Greif, Tel Aviv University

An international research team has taken a seat on the backs of wild bats to experience their world through echoes recorded by 3-gram computers.

In a new paper published in Science Advances, the research team from Aarhus University and the Max Planck Institute of Ornithology attached - and motion-recording tags to wild greater mouse-eared bats in Bulgaria.

"We experienced the world through the ears of the bats by recording their echoes directly on-board while they were hunting for insects at night," said Dr. Laura Stidsholt, postdoc at Aarhus University and leading author of the study.

Whispering bats

The tags recorded the echolocation calls and the movement of each bat in three dimensions, but most importantly, the echoes returning from their environment during one full night of foraging. This allowed the research team to tap into the sensory scenes of a hunting animal.

"We wanted to use the tags to find out how bats control what they 'see' when they hunt tiny insects on the wing on superfast timescales. We used the to find and track echoes from and vegetation, and to our surprise, we found that the bats are guided by extremely weak prey echoes that would be like a whisper to us," said Dr. Laura Stidsholt.

The bats themselves control the strength of their returning echoes by calling louder or weaker. So why would they choose these weak echoes, if they could increase the levels by calling louder?

An international research team has experienced the world of wild bats to experience through echoes recorded on-board the bats by 3-gram computers. The tiny computers also recorded the echolocation calls and the movement of each bat in three dimensions. With surprising results: The bats control the strength of their returning echoes by calling louder or weaker - and when hunting, they merely whisper to focus only on the prey and reduce the clutter from other echoes. Credit: Dr. Laura Stidsholt and Stefan Greif

Acoustic tunnel vision

To answer this, the researchers quantified the volume of air in which bats could potentially detect an echo for each echolocation call. The bats controlled the size of these sensory volumes by adjusting the strength of their calls.

"We found that hunting bats narrow their sensory volumes by more than a thousand times to focus only on the prey and thereby reduce the clutter from other echoes. It's like an acoustic version of a tunnel vision that briefly makes their world much simpler," said Dr. Holger Goerlitz of Max Planck Institute of Ornithology, a co-author of the study. He continued:

"The weak prey echoes might therefore be a consequence of the small sensory volumes shaped to hunt close to background clutter."

To protect these weak echoes from interference, the research team also showed that the bats used their flight patterns to separate the prey echoes from the background e.g. by flying parallel to trees.

The bat maneuvers fast in 3D to adjust its behavior to the sensory information from the surroundings. A female greater mouse-eared bat use echolocation to hunt for insects in air. Here is a 1 minute capture sequence, where she finally gets her meal indicated by chewing sounds in the end. Credit: Laura Stidsholt

Precise flight adjustments

"When the bats are hunting, they stay at least a prey detection distance away from the vegetation. We think they do this to avoid masking of the weak prey echoes by the loud echoes from vegetation. By continually adjusting both their flight patterns and their sensory volumes during the hunt, the bats simplify the information they need to process," said senior author Professor Peter Teglberg Madsen of Aarhus University.

Peter Teglberg Madsen suggests that the bats dedicate their attention and brain to the most essential information—getting their next meal—and that this might be one of the reasons that bats are such efficient hunters.

The miniature tags were designed and developed by Associate Professor Mark Johnson at Aarhus Institute of Advanced Sciences.

Release and capture. Credit: Stefan Greif

Small recorders—big challenge

"It was a real challenge to make a computer so small that it could work on a flying bat and still be sensitive enough to pick up these weak sounds," said Mark Johnson.

Close up of the tag. Credit: Stefan Greif

Echolocating bats account for 20 percent of all mammal species and play important roles in ecosystems across the planet.

"We think that this strategy has expanded the niches available to look for insects and is one of the reasons that bats have become such versatile hunters, but we don't know if they are versatile enough to cope with all the changes humans are making to the environment," said Professor Peter Teglberg Madsen.

More information: L. Stidsholt el al., "Hunting bats adjust their echolocation to receive weak prey echoes for clutter reduction," Science Advances (2021). advances.sciencemag.org/lookup … .1126/sciadv.abf1367

Journal information: Science Advances

Provided by Aarhus University