In small groups, people follow high-performing leaders

February 21, 2019, NYU Tandon School of Engineering
In small groups, people follow high-performing leaders
An example of network evolution in small groups. The directed links, those with arrows, were generated when one individual changed the answer to that of another. (a) When participants were shown the cumulative performance of others based on the last answers at each round in the main experiment, (b) when participants were shown the cumulative performance of others based on the first answers at each round in the additional experiment. Arrow width identifies the number of such instances, while the node size and colour represent network centrality and performance as a cumulative score, respectively. Credit: NYU Tandon School of Engineering

While the "wisdom of the crowd" shapes the behavior of large groups of people, less is known about small-group dynamics and how individuals interact to make decisions, particularly when it comes to the emergence of leaders, a key area of inquiry in organizational research. The phenomenon is critical to arriving at an understanding of social networks of all kinds.

Now, researchers at the NYU Tandon School of Engineering have cracked the code on how leaders arise from small groups of people over time. The work is detailed in a study, "Social information and Spontaneous Emergence of Leaders in Human Groups," published in The Royal Society Interface.

The team comprised Maurizio Porfiri, professor in the of Department of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering and of biomedical engineering at NYU Tandon; Shinnosuke Nakayama, postdoctoral researcher at NYU Tandon; Elizabeth Krasner, an at NYU Tandon; and Lorenzo Zino, a visiting Ph.D. student at NYU Tandon from Politecnico di Torino, Italy.

To conduct the research, the team convened several groups of five volunteers each to participate in a cognitive test arranged in 10 consecutive rounds. The task involved estimating the number of dots displayed for just half a second on a large screen. In each round, participants were asked to choose one from multiple answers using a custom-made clicker, without verbally communicating with one another. Because the dots were visible for only an instant, group members, lacking the time to count them, had to venture a guess. However, the experiments were structured so that participants could alter their answers based on the answers of others in their group: once all participants had chosen their initial answers, the screen—viewable by all—displayed the current answers of all members along with their past performance in selecting correct responses. Participants then had a 10-second window in which to change their responses based on those of the others in the group.

The researchers, analyzing how participant responses evolved over the course of the experiment, found that individuals did not choose the simple majority rule, as posited by the wisdom of crowds. Rather, they dynamically decided whom to follow in making decisions, based on how well each group member performed over time. Based on their observations, the researchers inferred a dynamic evolution of the network of interaction, in which participants were nodes and the links were the consequences of social influence. For example, the investigators generated a link from one participant to another if the first had changed his or her answer to that of the second. The speed at which the network grew increased over the course of each of the rounds.

"Individuals used social information more and more over time, and the more accurate the information, the more influence it had over participants' choices," said Porfiri. "Therefore, the relationship between participants' performance and their social influence was reinforced over time, resulting in the emergence of group leaders."

To discern the influence of social networks within evolving group dynamics, the investigators noted that:

  • Participants were influenced by in changing their answers. On average, participants changed answers to ones that nobody had selected only about 5 percent of the time. (There were more than 9 instances over all rounds in which participants changed answers to those of others and only 0.6 in which they changed answers to those no one had selected.)
  • Participants were more likely to be copied by others if their performances were good, even if their answers differed from those of the group majority.

Nakayama, the lead author, explained that the behavior of small groups is strikingly different than that of much larger gatherings of people.

"Where a large crowd would adopt a simple majority rule, with an increase in the accuracy of performance over repeated interactions, individuals rely more on social than personal information and as a consequence, good performers would emerge as leaders, exerting a stronger influence on others over time, "he said.

Porfiri noted that while most studies of social connections are based on static networks, defined by fixed established relationships, his team's research looked at functional networks, based on constantly changing connections.

"Historically, people have looked at social networks as equivalent to an anatomical based on static ties between people. We are putting forward the idea that in small groups, networks evolve in time based on actions," he said, adding that these time-based actions are like networks in the brain, where physically distant neurons forge connections toward a specific function. "Our approach is analogous to learning about neural circuits based on how they function in the brain, rather than how they are anatomically connected."

Explore further: Information sharing interferes with 'wisdom of crowds': study

More information: Shinnosuke Nakayama et al, Social information and spontaneous emergence of leaders in human groups, Journal of The Royal Society Interface (2019). DOI: 10.1098/rsif.2018.0938

Related Stories

Can social interactions affect spread of disease?

December 11, 2018

Most real-world systems, such as biological, social, and economic schemes evolve constantly. The dynamics of such systems are characterized by significantly enhanced activity levels over short periods of time (or "bursts") ...

Recommended for you

Physicists discover new class of pentaquarks

March 26, 2019

Tomasz Skwarnicki, professor of physics in the College of Arts and Sciences at Syracuse University, has uncovered new information about a class of particles called pentaquarks. His findings could lead to a new understanding ...

Study finds people who feed birds impact conservation

March 26, 2019

People in many parts of the world feed birds in their backyards, often due to a desire to help wildlife or to connect with nature. In the United States alone, over 57 million households in the feed backyard birds, spending ...

Matter waves and quantum splinters

March 25, 2019

Physicists in the United States, Austria and Brazil have shown that shaking ultracold Bose-Einstein condensates (BECs) can cause them to either divide into uniform segments or shatter into unpredictable splinters, depending ...

1 comment

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

Jayarava
not rated yet Feb 22, 2019
So people are naturally meritocratic?

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.