Remembering so as not to forget

Jul 20, 2010
Remembering so as not to forget

Verbal distractions are a primary cause of poor memory, according to scientific tests, which prove that the key to preventing ourselves from forgetting is to rehearse and ‘refresh’ our thoughts.

Psychologists from the University of Bristol conducted a series of tests with 117 six year old and 104 eight year old children to assess why we forget and how we preserve material in working memory, which governs our ability to process information in order to complete everyday tasks such as problem solving and arithmetic.

The findings - based on the first tests of their kind in children and revealed in the Journal of Experimental Child Psychology - indicate that the ability to preserve information in working memory begins at a much younger age than had previously been thought.  The results could have important implications for improving educational development. 

Both age groups were subjected to the same series of tests in which children were presented with sets of words to remember.  Some of these tests required children to delay their response and wait before recalling these words, and in some cases this delay involved an additional task, constituting a .  These distraction tests either involved verbal or nonverbal distraction, allowing researchers to assess how these would affect recall.

Any type of distraction adversely affected children’s recall, suggesting that without a distraction task the children ‘refreshed’ their memories of the original items during the delays.  However, verbal distraction was particularly damaging to recall, indicating that this presented children from carrying out ‘“phonological rehearsal” - verbally repeating the names of the items to themselves.

Professor Chris Jarrold, an expert in from the University of Bristol’s Department of , who led the research, said: “Our results confirm that we forget information from memory because we are distracted by other activities, but they also show that it is the type and not the amount of that distraction that really determines how much we forget. 

“Instead, when we are trying to remember verbal information such as a list of names or numbers, verbal distraction leads to greater forgetting than non-verbal distraction.  This is the case even when different distraction tasks require an equal amount of time as the original task in hand. This suggests that one way in which we try to remember information is by verbally rehearsing it, and the findings from our studies with children show that the ability to rehearse in this way develops considerably between the ages of six and eight.

“We know that children’s skills are closely related to their academic abilities in subjects like reading and maths.  One reason for this might be because these tasks require children to rehearse information in the face of potential distraction.”

Explore further: Intervention program helps prevent high-school dropouts

More information: Tam, H., Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., & Sabatos-DeVito, M. (in press). The development of memory maintenance: Six- and 8-year-olds' use of phonological rehearsal and attentional refreshment in working memory tasks, published in the Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. Access the paper online via the following link: dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.00

add to favorites email to friend print save as pdf

Related Stories

Brain fends off distractions

Mar 20, 2007

Dutch researcher Harm Veling has demonstrated that our brains fend off distractions. If we are busy with something we suppress disrupting external influences. If we are tired, we can no longer do this.

In Short-Term Memory, Faster Is Not Better, Study Shows

Nov 07, 2005

For years, researchers have said that the short-term memory increases as children mature, which is important because the memory serves as an index for intelligence and mental maturation. It has been proposed that a person's ...

Memory decline linked to an inability to ignore distractions

Mar 25, 2010

One of the most common complaints among healthy older adults relates to a decline in memory performance. This decline has been linked to an inability to ignore irrelevant information when forming memories. In order to ignore ...

Virtual game helps children escape realities of burn unit

Oct 09, 2007

Nurses and physicians at Nationwide Children’s Hospital are using the latest technology to help young burn victims endure the extreme pain of dressing changes and wound care. Instead of traditional distraction ...

Do doodle: Research shows doodling can help memory recall

Feb 27, 2009

Doodling while listening can help with remembering details, rather than implying that the mind is wandering as is the common perception. According to a study published today in the journal Applied Cognitive Psychology, subjec ...

Recommended for you

Intervention program helps prevent high-school dropouts

Oct 24, 2014

New research findings from a team of prevention scientists at Arizona State University demonstrates that a family-focused intervention program for middle-school Mexican American children leads to fewer drop-out rates and ...

Bilingualism over the lifespan

Oct 24, 2014

It's a scene that plays out every day in Montreal. On the bus, in schools, in the office and at home, conversations weave seamlessly back and forth between French and English, or one of the many other languages represented ...

User comments : 0