Cognition Without Control

Feb 24, 2010

(PhysOrg.com) -- Kids' inability to filter out distractions is a good thing. In fact, it's precisely what makes them such prodigious learners of language.

Teachers work hard to get and keep the attention of a classroom full of kids, and on the night shift, parents put lots of effort into keeping their flitting and impetuous brains on track with homework. Sometimes it can seem a wonder that kids learn anything at all. We might think grownups are the better learners, but remember that toddlers pick up all the unspoken rules and complex utterances of language better and faster than adults learning a new language. By age four, a child is speaking and has already learned many things about the world. That's the real wonder.

In "Cognition Without Control: When a Little Goes a Long Way," neuroscientist Sharon Thompson-Schill argues that kids' inability to filter out distractions is a good thing. In fact, she argues, it's precisely what makes them such prodigious learners of language. Written with a scientist from Stanford and a postdoc from her lab and published in Current Directions in , the paper looks at how immature, driven-to-distraction brain structures make possible the learning of complex social and linguistic conventions.

Thompson-Schill, the Class of 1965 Term Professor of Psychology, studies the biological bases of human cognitive systems—perception, memory, language, thought, cognitive control—and the interrelations among these systems. She is an expert on the brain bases of memory and language in humans. Her research focuses mainly on semantic memory—recall of knowledge about objects, facts, concepts, and words and their meanings. Using imaging and other brain-science techniques, she seeks to understand how and where in the frontal lobes semantic information is selectively and flexibly retrieved.

When a baby is born, "a massive proliferation of synapses" gets underway in the immature brain, Thompson-Schill says. branch across the cerebral cortex, chemical reactions begin churning, and sugar to feed the growing brain becomes metabolized. Unlike in other primates, different regions of the human brain mature at different rates. The visual and auditory cortex develops within a few months, but the prefrontal cortex, the part of the brain that governs attention and disciplines impulse, doesn't catch up in synaptic density until age four.

Because of this long period of brain development, Thompson-Schill says, children show impaired behavioral and cognitive control just like people with injuries to their prefrontal cortex. But, she adds, those underdeveloped powers of attention in kids are just what's needed for language learning. "Ever since the discovery that young children have a slowly developing frontal cortex—and the observation that this results in behavior that looks a lot like neurological patients who sustain damage to their frontal lobes—the focus has been on the behavioral cost of this developmental trajectory. We are interested in shifting the focus of this research to the possible benefits. That is, instead of asking, 'What can children do in spite of their immature frontal cortex?' we ask, ‘What can children do because of their immature frontal cortex?'"

Unlike the adult prefrontal cortex, an immature one is often not "on task" but is more open to absorbing raw experience, unfiltered by any plan, goal or agenda. "Because of this difference in frontal control," Thompson-Schill contends, "young children learn differently than adults." An undeveloped cortex allows young language learners to quickly pick up and put together words in the right order—more or less—and in the right context. They may have some initial trouble with exceptions to language rules—saying "mouses" instead of "mice," for example—but they hear and apply the main conventions for speaking because their brains are not under the control of some overriding goal, such as looking for and substituting exceptions.

The delayed maturing of frontal lobes may be evolution's way of programming the human brain to learn early the very things—language and social conventions—that make us human. The cost of prolonged development, Thompson-Schill notes, is a longer period of vulnerability for human offspring, but the trade-off is the rough but rapid learning that turns infants into human beings. "The benefits of a long period of 'hypofrontality,'" she argues, "outweigh the costs."

If the theory is correct, programs to develop self-control in the very young might not be such a good thing, Thompson-Schill cautions. "Young children might benefit more from informal, undirected learning," she says, pointing to how Brazilian kids learn the skills of soccer simply by kicking the ball around, while Americans typically learn through drills in soccer classes. "In other words, by considering the function of the developmental trajectory, a more balanced picture of costs and benefits emerges, and then an effort can be made to capitalize on the abilities that are enhanced by hypofrontality rather than simply focusing on correcting the abilities that are hindered by this state."

Explore further: Know the brain, and its axons, by the clothes they wear

add to favorites email to friend print save as pdf

Related Stories

Stroke study sheds light on left-right brain divide

Jun 11, 2007

Research into the effects of strokes has furthered our understanding of the different roles of the left and right sides of our brains. A study led by the University of Exeter has highlighted differences in the ability of ...

Recommended for you

Know the brain, and its axons, by the clothes they wear

Apr 18, 2014

(Medical Xpress)—It is widely know that the grey matter of the brain is grey because it is dense with cell bodies and capillaries. The white matter is almost entirely composed of lipid-based myelin, but ...

Turning off depression in the brain

Apr 17, 2014

Scientists have traced vulnerability to depression-like behaviors in mice to out-of-balance electrical activity inside neurons of the brain's reward circuit and experimentally reversed it – but there's ...

Rapid whole-brain imaging with single cell resolution

Apr 17, 2014

A major challenge of systems biology is understanding how phenomena at the cellular scale correlate with activity at the organism level. A concerted effort has been made especially in the brain, as scientists are aiming to ...

User comments : 0

More news stories

Treating depression in Parkinson's patients

A group of scientists from the University of Kentucky College of Medicine and the Sanders-Brown Center on Aging has found interesting new information in a study on depression and neuropsychological function in Parkinson's ...

Airbnb rental site raises $450 mn

Online lodging listings website Airbnb inked a $450 million funding deal with investors led by TPG, a source close to the matter said Friday.

Health care site flagged in Heartbleed review

People with accounts on the enrollment website for President Barack Obama's signature health care law are being told to change their passwords following an administration-wide review of the government's vulnerability to the ...