When crows connect

November 4, 2015
Corvus moneduloides, New Caledonian Crow. Image: Wikipedia.

An international team led by scientists at the University of St Andrews has studied social networks to understand how information might spread within and between groups of tool-using New Caledonian crows, according to a paper published in Nature Communications today (Tuesday 3 November 2015).

The New Caledonian crow is well-known for its ability to make and use tools to winkle nutritious insects out of their hiding places.

Dr James St Clair, lead author of the study, explains: "Tool-use is unusual in animals, and requires special knowledge. Individuals not only need to know how to make tools, but also where, when and how to actually use them. Crows could perhaps learn this sort of information from their neighbours, so we looked at how skills might spread among groups of birds."

To achieve this, the researchers analysed the social interactions of wild New Caledonian in their tropical habitat. Each crow was fitted with a high-tech, miniature spy tag, which communicated with tags on other crows and provided a continuous record of who met whom at any given time.

After recording crows' encounters during 'natural' conditions, the scientists altered the environment to see how this would affect the social network.

Dr Christian Rutz, team leader and co-author of the study, said: "Because we were interested in tool-use behaviours, we reasoned that hard-to-reach food would bring the crows together, providing extra opportunities to learn new skills from one another."

The team provided decaying timber full of wood-boring beetle grubs, a resource glut that can also occur naturally when dead trees fall and break open.

Having recorded the social network before, during and after this experiment, the researchers then ran computer simulations to examine how information might spread.

Dr Rutz noted: "We found that providing food has a similar effect to putting a coffee machine or water-cooler in an office – individuals aggregate around the resource, and the spread of interesting information is accelerated!"

Professor Rob Fleischer from the Smithsonian Conservation Biology Institute in Washington DC, USA, who led the genetic analyses for the project, added: "It was exciting to see how non-family birds suddenly started aggregating under these conditions."

Until recently, it was so difficult to record social interactions of wild animals that it could take weeks or months of data collection before scientists could build a social network. This meant that they were effectively blind to changes in network structure occurring over shorter timescales.

Physicist and social network expert Dr Dick James from the University of Bath, who co-authored the study, said: "By using tags to record actual associations, minute-by-minute, we were able to explore network dynamics over very short timescales. This, combined with the large volume of data that our tags delivered, brings the study of animal social networks a step closer to large-scale studies of human interactions, in which mobile phone data or Facebook posts are used to build incredibly detailed 'friendship' networks."

It has been suggested that, like humans and some great apes, New Caledonian crows have technological 'cultures'. Scientists still don't know how much of their tool-use behaviour crows learn from one another. However the current study, funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC) has demonstrated that opportunities for information exchange abound, especially when important resources encourage crows to forage in the same place.

New Caledonian crows live on the remote tropical island of New Caledonia in the South Pacific, where fieldwork for the present study was conducted. The tags used in this study are known as 'proximity loggers' or 'transceiver tags'. This is the first time that proximity logging has been done with wild birds. Proximity loggers have been used previously on larger animals – such as zebras, cattle or badgers – but until now, tags were far too heavy for flying birds.

For the study, 41 crows were fitted with tags, with each unit weighing only about 9g – much less than a £2 coin (12g). The tags were mounted onto the birds as 'backpacks', using harnesses that degrade over time. Over a 19-day period, more than 177,000 association logs were retrieved from 34 birds, with each log reporting metrics of bird-to-bird proximity and encounter duration. Genetic analyses were conducted by Professor Rob Fleischer and Dr Brandt Ryder at the Smithsonian Conservation Biology Institute, Washington DC, USA.

In 2012, Dr Christian Rutz's research group reported preliminary results of the project in Current Biology. The present study expands on this earlier work by presenting experimental manipulations, in-depth social network analysis, and simulations of information flow.

Explore further: Crows, like humans, store their tools when not in use

More information: James J. H. St Clair et al. Experimental resource pulses influence social-network dynamics and the potential for information flow in tool-using crows, Nature Communications (2015). DOI: 10.1038/ncomms8197

Related Stories

Crows, like humans, store their tools when not in use

May 21, 2015

Researchers at the University of St Andrews have discovered that crows, like humans, store their tools when they don't need them. The study published today in the Proceedings of the Royal Society B is the first to examine ...

Mapping the social networking of birds

September 12, 2012

(Phys.org)—A team, led by researchers at the Universities of St Andrews and Washington, used novel radio-transmitters to study the social networks of New Caledonian crows, a species renowned for using sophisticated foraging ...

Canny crows know their tools

October 8, 2013

(Phys.org) —Scientists at the University of St Andrews have discovered that New Caledonian crows, famous for their use of tools to extract hidden food, do not rely on guesswork when deploying one of their most complicated ...

New Caledonian crows show strong evidence of social learning

August 26, 2015

Among our greatest achievements as humans, some might say, is our cumulative technological culture—the tool-using acumen that is passed from one generation to the next. As the implements we use on a daily basis are modified ...

Crows show advanced learning abilities

December 14, 2011

New Caledonian crows have, in the past, distinguished themselves with their advanced tool using abilities. A team of researchers from the University of Auckland and the University of Cambridge have now shown these crows can ...

Wild crows reveal tool skills

January 11, 2010

(PhysOrg.com) -- A new study using motion sensitive video cameras has revealed how New Caledonian crows use tools in the wild, Oxford University scientists report.

Recommended for you

New analysis of big data sheds light on cell functions

October 26, 2016

Researchers have developed a new way of obtaining useful information from big data in biology to better understand—and predict—what goes on inside a cell. Using genome-scale models, researchers were able to integrate ...

Researchers identify genes for 'Help me!' aromas from corn

October 25, 2016

When corn seedlings are nibbled by caterpillars, they defend themselves by releasing scent compounds that attract parasitic wasps whose larvae consume the caterpillar—but not all corn varieties are equally effective at ...

Structure of key DNA replication protein solved

October 25, 2016

A research team led by scientists at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (ISMMS) has solved the three-dimensional structure of a key protein that helps damaged cellular DNA repair itself. Investigators say that knowing ...

Genome editing: Efficient CRISPR experiments in mouse cells

October 25, 2016

In order to use the CRISPR-Cas9 system to cut genes, researchers must design an RNA sequence that matches the DNA of the target gene. Most genes have hundreds of such sequences, with varying activity and uniqueness in the ...


Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

5 / 5 (1) Nov 06, 2015
Is "winkle" standard English?
Captain Stumpy
5 / 5 (2) Nov 06, 2015
Is "winkle" standard English?
Yes. though chiefly British, you can also find the term used in certain regions in the US as well...
transitive verb
1- chiefly British : to displace, remove, or evict from a position —usually used with out
2- chiefly British : to obtain or draw out by effort —usually used with out
Origin of WINKLE - from the process of extracting a winkle from its shell
First Known Use: 1918

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.