New study shows how complex bonobo communication is similar to that of human infants

New study shows how complex bonobo communication is similar to that of human infants
Wild bonobos. Credit: Zanna Clay/Lui Kotale Bonobo Project

Researchers at the University of Birmingham, UK and the University of Neuchatel, Switzerland, have found that wild bonobos, our closest living relatives in the primate world, communicate in a similar manner to human infants, using a high-pitched call type, or 'peep', that requires context to be understood.

The finding that bonobos use a type of call, that alters meaning depending on context, echoes the context independent manner in which can also communicate.

Bonobos make 'peep' calls which are short, very high-pitched and produced with a closed mouth. They produce these calls in a wide range of situations, across positive, negative and neutral circumstances.

The researchers looked at the acoustic structure and found that the peep calls did not vary acoustically between neutral and positive contexts - for example, between feeding, travelling and resting.

It is this similarity in calls made across different emotional contexts that echo the similarities found in human infant vocalisations.

Dr Zanna Clay, from the University of Birmingham's School of Psychology, who led the study, said: 'When I studied the bonobos in their native setting in Congo, I was struck by how frequent their peeps were, and how many different contexts they produce them in. It became apparent that because we couldn't always differentiate between peeps, we needed understand the context to get to the root of their communication.'

The common assumption is that primate calls are tightly tied to certain contexts and emotional states, whereas many human vocalisations are freed from this.

New study shows how complex bonobo communication is similar to that of human infants
Wild bonobos. Credit: Zanna Clay/Lui Kotale Bonobo Project

However, before language develops, human babies can produce a group of calls- called 'protophones'- independently of their . These types of infant calls differ from commonly recognised calls such as laughter and crying, and the calls of most animals, which are thought only to be produced in certain contexts.

Dr Clay added, "We felt that it was premature to conclude that this ability is uniquely human, especially as noone had really looked for it in the great apes. It appears that the more we look, the more similarity we find between animals and humans."

New study shows how complex bonobo communication is similar to that of human infants
Wild bonobos. Credit: Zanna Clay/Lui Kotale Bonobo Project

The type of communication the researchers observed in the wild could represent an important evolutionary transition from 'functionally fixed animal vocalisations' - vocalisations that are tied to specific contexts or emotional states - towards vocalisations, which seems to have appeared some 6 - 10 million years ago with the bonobo, our shared common ancestor with apes.

The research, published in the journal PeerJ, also found that the peeps made in emotionally negative circumstances, such as an aggressive encounter, or in response to predators, demonstrate a pattern more typical to primates in that the acoustic structure is more closely tied to certain emotional states.

More information: Clay et al. (2015), Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour. PeerJ 3:e1124; DOI: 10.7717/peerj.1124

Journal information: PeerJ

Citation: New study shows how complex bonobo communication is similar to that of human infants (2015, August 4) retrieved 19 March 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2015-08-complex-bonobo-similar-human-infants.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

The hoo's hoo of gibbon communication

1002 shares

Feedback to editors