Songbirds have a thing for patterns

songbird
Eastern Yellow Robin. Credit: Wikipedia.

You might think that young children would first learn to recognize sounds and then learn how those categories of sounds fit together into words. But that isn't how it works. Rather, kids learn sounds and words at the same time. In fact, the higher-level patterns—those words—are key in learning to recognize and place speech sounds into meaningful categories. That's why children who grow up in Japanese-speaking homes don't recognize the difference between 'r' and 'l' sounds. In Japanese, that distinction doesn't matter. Now, researchers reporting in the Cell Press journal Current Biology on June 25 present evidence from European starlings showing that songbirds learn their songs in much the same way.

"It wasn't clear whether this kind of learning—where knowledge of a pattern informs understanding about the categories that make up the patterns—was unique to humans and language, or is a more general process shared with other animals," says Timothy Gentner of the University of California, San Diego. "We showed that this kind of learning is shared with, at least, songbirds. When birds learn to recognize patterns of song elements, they get a big boost in their ability to categorize those elements."

Starlings' songs consist of short, stereotyped patterns of simple notes grouped into 'motifs' and longer, well-defined sequences of motifs organized into bouts. Those motifs fall into one of four categories: whistles, warbles, rattles, or high frequencies. Gentner and his colleagues wanted to find out how patterns influence starlings in their ability to categorize those sounds.

Jordan Comins, a graduate student in Gentner's lab and lead author of the study, trained four starlings to differentiate complex auditory patterns following the form AABB and BBAA from those that followed the form ABAB and BABA, where A and B denote natural motif categories of warbles and rattles. The researchers trained a second group in the same way, except that they shuffled the patterns.

The researchers found that the birds presented with clear patterns of sounds were at a big advantage over the other group in their ability to correctly categorize the motifs they'd heard. The findings show that rely on patterns in much the same way that people do.

Starlings heard a sequence of four elements, each following a different pattern made up of mostly the same elements. The birds had to learn to classify the whole sequence according to the pattern and were then later challenged to categorize the elements. Credit: Timothy Gentner
Starlings heard a sequence of four elements, each following a different pattern made up of mostly the same elements. The birds had to learn to classify the whole sequence according to the pattern and were then later challenged to categorize the elements. Credit: Timothy Gentner

"We 'hear' words, not sequences of ," Gentner says. "While it is tempting to think that this is a uniquely human way of perceiving the world, it is not."

Birds may not have language, but they still can teach us much about the biological and psychological mechanisms that make language and possible, the researchers say. Having identified this basic form of shared top-down learning, Gentner's team will now explore more subtle details of the perceptual categories and in the birds to see how precisely they match human speech acquisition. They will also examine the extent to which pattern knowledge shapes the representation of complex vocal signals in the songbird brain.

More information: Current Biology, Comins and Gentner: 'Pattern-induced covert category learning in songbirds' dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.046

Journal information: Current Biology

Provided by Cell Press

Citation: Songbirds have a thing for patterns (2015, June 25) retrieved 19 March 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2015-06-songbirds-patterns.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Discovery helps to unlock brain's speech-learning mechanism

58 shares

Feedback to editors