Butterfly collection pinpoints brilliance of mimicry

July 15, 2014 by Nicola Pearson
Butterfly collection pinpoints brilliance of mimicry
Morphs of P. dardanus from a specimen tray in the Museum collection. Credit: PLoS

New research on the Museum's Lepidoptera collection has shed light on how some butterflies pretend to be lethal.

For decades, biologists have been intrigued by the staggering diversity of within certain butterfly , in particular the mocker swallowtail, Papilio dardanus.

The array of different forms exhibited by this African butterfly, now estimated at between 11 and 18, meant that until 1869, they were often described as belonging to separate species.

Fooling the enemy

P. dardanus is a textbook example of a successful Batesian mimic - an edible butterfly species in which the females take on the colours and wing patterns of toxic species to ward off predators.

Successful mimics such as P. dardanus cannot afford to become more abundant than the species they're copying otherwise predators, such as baby birds, learn that they're safe to eat.

This means its appearance continually evolves to maintain the wariness of predators.

In a new study published this week in Plos One, Cambridge University PhD researcher Martin Thompson databased and geocoded the 3,395 wild specimens preserved in the Museum's pinned collection to help explain the existence of 'imperfect' intermediates - non-mimetic female , believed to have resulted from a breakdown of the mimetic pattern.

Broken brilliance

'In other words, we wanted to see if something goes wrong in certain geographical locations that breaks down the dominance hierarchy and makes some of the butterflies no longer mimics,' Thompson said.

The collection shows is a clear divergence between butterfly populations in East and West Africa, across the rift valley, suggesting the butterflies adapt to separate environments with cross-breeding in the middle producing unpredictable outcomes.

It could be, however, that being an inaccurate mimic in these areas does not put the butterflies at risk because of a lack of predators, or it could simply mean there are a fewer distasteful species to mimic.

Thompson said the Museum's Lepidoptera collection allowed the team to plot the hotspots for the different P. dardanus wing patterns, for example the area of East Africa surrounding Lake Victoria in Uganda and Kenya, which hopefully will lead to more work to understand the genetics behind successful mimicry.

Sensational Butterflies on display

The team behind the Plos One paper is about to publish further research on the DNA of P. dardanus supplied by specimens that successfully emerged in the Museum's live butterfly house last year.

Although a tropical butterfly, there are currently live P. dardanus in the Museum's Sensational Butterflies exhibition, with more hopefully about to emerge.

Other swallowtails belonging to the Papilio group can also be seen in the butterfly house, which is open to the public until 14 September 2014.

There is also a display of P. dardanus specimens in Dinosaur Way in the Blue Zone.

Explore further: Dual-sex butterfly hatches at Natural History Museum

More information: Thompson MJ, Timmermans MJTN (2014) "Characterising the Phenotypic Diversity of Papilio dardanus Wing Patterns Using an Extensive Museum Collection." PLoS ONE 9(5): e96815. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096815

Related Stories

'Supergene' is key to copycat butterflies

August 12, 2011

Since Charles Darwin, biologists have pondered the mystery of "mimicry butterflies", which survive by copying the wing patterns of other butterflies that taste horrible to their predators, birds.

Climate change may disrupt butterfly flight seasons

November 21, 2013

The flight season timing of a wide variety of butterflies is responsive to temperature and could be altered by climate change, according to a UBC study that leverages more than a century's worth of museum and weather records.

Recommended for you

Scientists overcome key CRISPR-Cas9 genome editing hurdle

December 1, 2015

Researchers at the Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard and the McGovern Institute for Brain Research at MIT have engineered changes to the revolutionary CRISPR-Cas9 genome editing system that significantly cut down on "off-target" ...

Study finds 'rudimentary' empathy in macaques

December 1, 2015

(Phys.org)—A pair of researchers with Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique and Université Lyon, in France has conducted a study that has shown that macaques have at least some degree of empathy towards their fellow ...

Which came first—the sponge or the comb jelly?

December 1, 2015

Bristol study reaffirms classical view of early animal evolution. Whether sponges or comb jellies (also known as sea gooseberries) represent the oldest extant animal phylum is of crucial importance to our understanding of ...

Trap-jaw ants exhibit previously unseen jumping behavior

December 1, 2015

A species of trap-jaw ant has been found to exhibit a previously unseen jumping behavior, using its legs rather than its powerful jaws. The discovery makes this species, Odontomachus rixosus, the only species of ant that ...


Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.