Greenhouse experiments show plant's long-term memory

Mar 27, 2014 by Rebecca Graham
“The little plants remembered one event of one day, one month later which was just amazing!”—Dr Gagliano. Credit: Jkadavoor

Scientists have demonstrated that Mimosa pudica plants not only learn from experience—they also remember what they have learnt over extended periods of time.

The groundbreaking study used the same experimental methods usually reserved for testing learned behavioural responses in animals.

Mimosa pudica, native to South and Central America, is known as the 'sensitive plant' due to its defensive leaf-folding reflex in response to physical stimuli.

Lead researcher Dr Monica Gagliano from UWA's Centre for Evolutionary Biology School of Animal Biology, was curious to explore Mimosa's capacity to develop learned through habituation.

"Habituation is where you actively learn to adapt to and filter out stimuli which have proven over time to be harmless, enabling you to remain responsive to your surrounding environment," she says.

The researchers devised an apparatus which dropped each potted Mimosa 15cm down a vertical rail onto a foam base, generating a physical shock that elicited the leaf-folding behaviour.

The plants were divided into a low-light (LL) and high-light (HL) environment, hypothesising the LL plants would be faster learners and retain their memory longer given their greater need for open leaves (for photosynthesis).

A single drop was administered to 16 control plants (eight per light condition) and again eight hours later; they swiftly closed their leaves both times.

The researchers then 'trained' 56 plants (28 per light condition) by administering 60 consecutive drops, five to 10 seconds apart, seven times within a day.

After the first four to six drops the plants habituated swiftly, keeping their leaves open after learning the drops presented no real threat.

As predicted, plants in LL re-opened their leaves more widely.

"They learn the same way we do … they acquire a new understanding of their environment and change their behaviour accordingly," Dr Gagliano says.

"They also change their behaviour depending on what the environment is demanding, so when the light was not at an optimum level, it became very important to work this out and adapt quickly."

Plants display long-term behaviour changes

Mimosa's long-term memory when exposed to new environments was tested, where plants from LL were switched to HL and vice versa, and re-tested 28 days later using the full-day training regime.

They continued to exhibit the learned behaviour in the new light condition, indicating long-term habituation in the face of changed environments.

"The plants' learnt behaviour is important in the present but also important in the future, so they don't waste energy repeating the same process of acquiring knowledge again," Dr Gagliano says.

"The little plants remembered one event of one day, one month later which was just amazing!"

She is interested in seeing what happens when more complex conditions and scenarios of learning are introduced to the .

Explore further: Move over elephants: Mimosas have memories too

More information: "Experience teaches plants to learn faster and forget slower in environments where it matters." Monica Gagliano, Michael Renton, Martial Depczynski, Stefano Mancuso. Oecologia. January 2014. link.springer.com/article/10.1… %2Fs00442-013-2873-7

add to favorites email to friend print save as pdf

Related Stories

Move over elephants: Mimosas have memories too

Jan 15, 2014

Not long after publishing a paper in a prestigious journal about plants being able to 'talk' using sound, Monica Gagliano is back with her new findings showing that they can 'learn'.

Get touchy feely with plants

Sep 16, 2013

Forget talking to plants to help them grow, gently rubbing them with your fingers can make them less susceptible to disease, a paper in the open access journal BMC Plant Biology reveals.

Plants 'talk' to plants to help them grow

May 06, 2013

Having a neighborly chat improves seed germination, finds research in BioMed Central's open access journal BMC Ecology. Even when other known means of communication, such as contact, chemical and light-mediated signal ...

Plants convert energy at lightning speed

Mar 03, 2014

A new way of measuring how much light a plant can tolerate could be useful in growing crops resilient to a changing climate, according to scientists from Queen Mary University of London.

Team discovers how plants avoid sunburn

Aug 06, 2013

A Dartmouth-led team has discovered a group of stress-related proteins that explains how plants avoid sunburn in intense light, a finding that one day could help biotechnologists to develop crops that can better cope with ...

Goats are far more clever than previously thought

Mar 25, 2014

Goats learn how to solve complicated tasks quickly and can recall how to perform them for at least 10 months, which might explain their remarkable ability to adapt to harsh environments, say researchers at ...

Recommended for you

Wolves susceptible to yawn contagion

13 hours ago

Wolves may be susceptible to yawn contagion, according to a study published August 27, 2014 in the open-access journal PLOS ONE by Teresa Romero from The University of Tokyo, Japan, and colleagues.

User comments : 1

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

Plantastic
5 / 5 (1) Mar 27, 2014
It is amazing how sensitive the TickleMe Plant is and how the leaves fold when Tickled.
TickleMe Plants are more like pets then plants. They are easy to grow indoors in the
TickleMe Plant Greenhouse
http://www.ticklemeplant.com