Insects master abstract concepts

May 03, 2012
A honeybee trained to choose stimuli according to the 'above/below' and 'difference' concepts. The bee chooses the stimuli composed of figures it has never seen in cases when they fulfill both concepts simultaneously: one is above the other and they are different. © A. Avargučs-Weber, CRCA

An insect's brain is capable of constructing and handling abstract concepts. It can even use two different concepts simultaneously in order to make a decision when faced with a new situation.

This unexpected result was obtained by Professor Martin Giurfa and his team at the Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale(CNRS/Université Toulouse III - Paul Sabatier). While it was previously thought that only humans and some primates were capable of such sophisticated cognitive analysis, these findings demonstrate that the absence of language and a tiny neural architecture do not hinder this ability. The team's work, published in the journalPNAS, calls into question many theories in fields such as animal cognition, human psychology, neurosciences and artificial intelligence.

Human cognition, notably our mathematical and linguistic capacities, is based on our ability to handle concepts1. In everyday life, concepts relating objects together according to their relationship with one another—such as 'different', 'the same', 'more than' or 'above'—play a predominant role. Car drivers, for example, rely on a complex network of concepts: color codes, arrows, signposts, etc., to find their way. The use of such concepts, which were long thought to be specific to man and certain primates, could be more widely spread though the animal kingdom than previously believed.

The researchers showed that honeybees are capable of constructing and handling concepts to reach a food source. They trained groups of bees to enter a chamber where two short corridors led to image stimuli. Each stimulus was composed of two distinct images placed either side by side (see photograph below) or above one another. A reservoir containing either a reward (consisting of a drop of sugar water), or a punishment (consisting of a drop of quinine) was placed between these pairs of images. The bees could be rewarded according to one concept (for example 'above/below') and punished for another ('side by side'), or vice versa. For each group, the images kept changing, but the relationships 'above/below' and 'side by side' and their respective associations with reward and punishment were maintained. After thirty training runs, the bees were able to recognize the relation that led to the sugar water.

One of the experiments consisted in presenting the same bees with new images. The only common characteristic between the images used during the training and those used for the test was their positioning: above one another or side by side. Even though the bees had never seen these new stimuli, they made correct choices, according to the abstract relationship underlying their position.

But that was not all that the study revealed. During training, the two images indicating the reward were always different from one another (as on the photograph below). To find out whether the bees had also learnt this concept of 'difference', the researchers presented them with new stimuli in which the images were in the reward position (for example, one above the other) but were either different or identical. The bees ignored the stimuli composed of identical images, showing that, in addition to the concepts of 'above/below' and 'side by side', they could simultaneously handle the concept of difference in order to take a decision.

The results of this study call into question the idea that mammalian brains (including our own), with their larger size, are necessary for conceptual knowledge. They also demonstrate that the formation of concepts is possible in the absence of language. From a philosophical point of view, it fuels the debate surrounding what is specific to mankind. Martin Giurfa's team is now working on the identification of the neuronal networks responsible for this conceptualization.

Explore further: Rising temperatures can be hard on dogs

More information: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, online 19 April 2012

add to favorites email to friend print save as pdf

Related Stories

Bees recognize human faces using feature configuration

Jan 29, 2010

Going about their day-to-day business, bees have no need to be able to recognise human faces. Yet in 2005, when Adrian Dyer from Monash University trained the fascinating insects to associate pictures of human ...

For stressed bees, the glass is half empty

Jun 02, 2011

When people are depressed or anxious, they are much more likely to see their glass as half empty than half full. In tough times, evidence of that same pessimistic outlook can be seen in dogs, rats, and birds. Now, researchers ...

New Insight Into How Bees See

Jan 23, 2009

New research from Monash University bee researcher Adrian Dyer could lead to improved artificial intelligence systems and computer programs for facial recognition.

Your left side is your best side

Apr 20, 2012

Your best side may be your left cheek, according to a new study by Kelsey Blackburn and James Schirillo from Wake Forest University in the US. Their work shows that images of the left side of the face are perceived and rated ...

Bees show sophisticated learning abilities

Mar 31, 2005

Honeybees have robust and flexible memory systems that enable them to apply abstract rules to solve novel problems, according to new ANU research. Although the brains of these insects are very small, over the ...

Recommended for you

Vietnam's taste for cat leaves pets in peril

18 minutes ago

The enduring popularity of "little tiger" as a snack to accompany a beer in Vietnam means that cat owners live in constant fear of animal snatchers, despite an official ban.

New species of mayfly discovered in India

1 hour ago

Scientists have discovered a new species of mayfly in the southern Western Ghats, a mountain range along the west coast of India. In fact, this is the first time that any mayfly belonging to the genus Labiobaetis has be ...

Rising temperatures can be hard on dogs

Jul 25, 2014

The "dog days of summer" are here, but don't let the phrase fool you. This hot time of year can be dangerous for your pup, says a Kansas State University veterinarian.

User comments : 4

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

Isaacsname
1 / 5 (1) May 03, 2012
Lol.

" This unexpected result "

Have people forgotten the existence of the correspondence principle ?

There are always going to be results you didn't expect, why is that a surprise ?

CHollman82
3 / 5 (2) May 03, 2012
There are always going to be results you didn't expect, why is that a surprise?


It is by definition. If they weren't a surprise then they wouldn't be unexpected (as these are basically synonyms) and your premise (there will always be unexpected results) would be false.
CHollman82
3 / 5 (2) May 03, 2012
I've always known that we give other life too little credit. Some of the things that the simplest insects do is downright amazing. I took an elective in school called "Insects and Society", I figured it was a throw-away class and an easy A, turned out to be one of the most interesting courses I've ever taken.
ormondotvos
1 / 5 (1) May 03, 2012
Couldn't just be caused by the structure of the image processing system, and the bees take the easiest path?