Study shows insect mimic abilities related to size

Mar 22, 2012 by Bob Yirka report
The relationship between Mahalanobis distances (f_MD) and human rankings (f_HR) measures of mimetic fidelity in hoverflies. Image (c) Nature 483, 461-464 (22 March 2012) doi:10.1038/nature10961

(PhysOrg.com) -- A group of Canadian researchers who found themselves wondering why some plants or animals are good mimics while others are not, has undertaken a study on the matter and believe they have found the answer. They suggest, as they write in their paper published in Nature, that it has to do with size. The bigger they are, the more payoff for those that wish to eat them, thus the need for better mimicry skills.

To come to this conclusion, the team looked at five informal that have been tossed around in the scientific community to explain the differences in mimicry skills displayed by different or , tested each and found holes in all of them but one. Size.

To carry out their testing, the team focused on the 18 different species of the hoverfly. This was a good choice because some species are very good at imitating or wasps and some are not. Also, they come in a variety of shapes and sizes.

One of the first challenges was to come up with a way to measure just how closely a hoverfly resembled the bee or wasp it was supposed to look like. To do this they showed photographs to volunteer humans who graded them on a scale of one to ten. They then merged those results with physical measurements taken of both the hoverflies and the bees or they most looked like, to arrive at a score for each species.

One of the theories that the researchers tested was the idea that what we humans see as poor mimicry skills may not be so for the that may wish to eat them. Another is that those that have poor skills wind up representing the worst of a group of different species, resulting in the of an unappetizing meal.

In reality, none of these ideas seemed to hold much water, as the team found that what really mattered was size. The bigger the bug, the better it was at mimicking a bee or wasp. Smaller hoverflies don’t need to be very good at looking like a bee or wasp that might sting birds that wish to eat them, because they don’t represent much payoff. Thus birds looking for a meal don’t bother looking very hard at them. If there is any chance of being stung, they simply move on. Larger hoverflies on the other hand, are worth a second or third look, thus they better be much more convincing.

Explore further: Orchid named after UC Riverside researcher

More information: A comparative analysis of the evolution of imperfect mimicry, Nature 483, 461–464 (22 March 2012) doi:10.1038/nature10961

Abstract
Although exceptional examples of adaptation are frequently celebrated, some outcomes of natural selection seem far from perfect. For example, many hoverflies (Diptera: Syrphidae) are harmless (Batesian) mimics of stinging Hymenoptera. However, although some hoverfly species are considered excellent mimics, other species bear only a superficial resemblance to their models and it is unclear why this is so. To evaluate hypotheses that have been put forward to explain interspecific variation in the mimetic fidelity of Palearctic Syrphidae we use a comparative approach. We show that the most plausible explanation is that predators impose less selection for mimetic fidelity on smaller hoverfly species because they are less profitable prey items. In particular, our findings, in combination with previous results, allow us to reject several key hypotheses for imperfect mimicry: first, human ratings of mimetic fidelity are positively correlated with both morphometric measures and avian rankings, indicating that variation in mimetic fidelity is not simply an illusion based on human perception; second, no species of syrphid maps out in multidimensional space as being intermediate in appearance between several different hymenopteran model species, as the multimodel hypothesis requires; and third, we find no evidence for a negative relationship between mimetic fidelity and abundance, which calls into question the kin-selection hypothesis. By contrast, a strong positive relationship between mimetic fidelity and body size supports the relaxed-selection hypothesis, suggesting that reduced predation pressure on less profitable prey species limits the selection for mimetic perfection.

Related Stories

Stress may explain vocal mimicry in Bowerbirds

May 11, 2011

(PhysOrg.com) -- Spotted Bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus maculatus) are best known for their nests, but these birds are also capable of mimicking the vocalizations of many different species of birds. It was be ...

How do bumblebees get predators to buzz off?

May 26, 2010

Toxic or venomous animals, like bumblebees, are often brightly coloured to tell would-be predators to keep away. However scientists at Royal Holloway, University of London and Queen Mary, University of London ...

Recommended for you

Orchid named after UC Riverside researcher

9 hours ago

One day about eight years ago, Katia Silvera, a postdoctoral scholar at the University of California, Riverside, and her father were on a field trip in a mountainous area in central Panama when they stumbled ...

In sex-reversed cave insects, females have the penises

12 hours ago

Researchers reporting in the Cell Press journal Current Biology on April 17 have discovered little-known cave insects with rather novel sex lives. The Brazilian insects, which represent four distinct but re ...

Fear of the cuckoo mafia

12 hours ago

If a restaurant owner fails to pay the protection money demanded of him, he can expect his premises to be trashed. Warnings like these are seldom required, however, as fear of the consequences is enough to ...

User comments : 1

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

Marquette
not rated yet Mar 22, 2012
"..five informal hypotheses..."

More news stories

Deadly human pathogen Cryptococcus fully sequenced

Within each strand of DNA lies the blueprint for building an organism, along with the keys to its evolution and survival. These genetic instructions can give valuable insight into why pathogens like Cryptococcus ne ...

Hackathon team's GoogolPlex gives Siri extra powers

(Phys.org) —Four freshmen at the University of Pennsylvania have taken Apple's personal assistant Siri to behave as a graduate-level executive assistant which, when asked, is capable of adjusting the temperature ...

Better thermal-imaging lens from waste sulfur

Sulfur left over from refining fossil fuels can be transformed into cheap, lightweight, plastic lenses for infrared devices, including night-vision goggles, a University of Arizona-led international team ...