Hunger may inhibit defensive behavior

Feb 01, 2012 By Tamera Jones
Hunger may inhibit defensive behavior
Adult southern pied babbler.

Most animals don't spend nearly as much time and energy defending nesting or mating sites against intruders outside the breeding season. That's a given.

But this might be less about having no need to, and more because food is at a premium and they have no choice but to save their energy.

That's the of the latest study to find out why so many creatures aren't as territorial when they're not breeding as when they're rearing .

While studying the territorial behavior of pied babblers in the southern African Kalahari during her PhD, Dr. Krystyna Golabek from the University of Bristol noticed big differences between breeding and non-breeding seasons.

During the breeding season the defend their territories to the hilt. Indeed, they get quite worked up, furiously flying at each other, sometimes knocking rivals to the ground.

"There are a lot of battles between neighbouring groups – they get quite vocal and get into fights, even though theory dictates fighting should be a last resort," says Dr. Andy Radford from the University of Bristol, Golabek's supervisor and co-author of the study.

"Krystyna was interested in what drives these seasonal differences. You don't see dramatic interactions between these birds throughout the year, only during the breeding season," explains Radford.

Many scientists tend to assume that most don't bother defending their territories when they're not breeding, because there's little point. Why bother defending nests or mating sites when you don't need them?

But Golabek and her colleagues reasoned that because defensive behaviour takes up so much time and energy, behavioral differences between seasons could be driven by a lack of food and a need to save energy.

Hunger may inhibit defensive behavior
Pied babbler.

"The usual assumption is that the difference in behaviour between seasons has an adaptive benefit: being defensive helps you keep the best nest sites for example," explains Radford.

"But we decided to look at it the other way round: is it because they're constrained in some way outside the breeding season?"

To test their ideas, the researchers analysed how fervently the Kalahari pied babblers defended their hard-won territories when they're not breeding.

They found that they spent little time interacting with , and barely reacted when faced with an intruder.

During her studies, Golabek also discovered that the birds aren't as successful at tracking down food outside the breeding season compared with when they're breeding. This was reflected in the birds' weights.

"The birds are trained to jump on a scale so we can weigh them," says Radford. "It's a great study system: you can run controlled experiments on wild populations."

But when the birds had a ready supply of food – even outside the breeding season – they significantly upped their defensive behaviour against a mock intrusion into their territories – squawking and flying at the perceived impostor.

"What nailed our conclusions was the discovery that the babblers got more defensive when they had access to food outside the ," Radford says.

The Kalahari pied babblers make a perfect study system. Mandy Ridley, then based at the University of Cape Town, set up the study population in 2003. She color-ringed the birds and got them used to people being around. So much so that researchers can now walk within a few feet of them, with no noticeable effect on their behavior.

Radford added that the birds could also be less defensive because intruders have less energy to challenge territories.

"Being defensive is often a reaction to an intruder. But both could be taking a hit: there could be a constraint on the as well," he adds.

"And this could be the same for primates, or other cooperative species. There's no reason to think this is bird-specific, because most are food-limited at some point during the year."

The study is published in Animal Behavior.

Golabek is currently studying in Botswana.

Explore further: Alternate mechanism of species formation picks up support, thanks to a South American ant

More information: K.A. Golabek, Food availability affects strength of seasonal territorial behaviour in a cooperatively breeding bird, Animal Behavior, published online 13 January 2012, doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.034

Related Stories

Feed the birds: Winter feeding makes for better breeding

Feb 06, 2008

Keep feeding the birds over winter: that’s the message from research by the University of Exeter and Queen’s University Belfast, published today (6 February 2008) in the Royal Society journal Biology Letters. The study ...

The watchman’s song

Jun 17, 2008

Soldiers on sentry duty in hostile territory keep in regular radio contact with their colleagues to assure them that all is well and that they are safe to carry on their manoeuvres. New research by Dr Andy ...

The burly bird catches the girl

Aug 17, 2011

While the early bird might catch the worm, it's the quick bird that lands the ladies, according to new research into the running performance of an Arctic cousin of the grouse.

Recommended for you

Orb-weaving spiders living in urban areas may be larger

Aug 20, 2014

A common orb-weaving spider may grow larger and have an increased ability to reproduce when living in urban areas, according to a study published August 20, 2014 in the open-access journal PLOS ONE by Eli ...

User comments : 0