Social behavior can make predators even pickier

Aug 18, 2011 By Adele Rackley
Social behavior can make predators even pickier

Your greedy cat may turn up his nose at different food, but wild animals can be conservative when it comes to food choices too. And new research suggests that, in a group, even adventurous individuals can end up acting like their more selective peers.

In this new study, a team led by researchers from Cardiff University wanted to test whether shoals of stickleback would behave differently when faced with new types of food. They got the fish used to eating water fleas that had been coloured brown or green, then introduced a 'novel' food in the shape of fleas of the other color.

In most cases the fish gobbled up the fleas indiscriminately. But a significant proportion played it safe and ate only the familiar color, allowing the novel flea population to flourish and eventually dominate.

This supports the idea that picky predators may strongly influence prey populations: unusual-looking prey are effectively protected from by their novelty, so numbers increase.

"The focus of this study was to examine whether influences individual foragers' behavior" explains Dr. Rob Thomas of Cardiff University, a co-author of the report published in Behavioral Ecology.

Some individual animals naturally avoid new foods - a foraging strategy known as 'dietary conservatism' (DC). Within a group there will always be animals that eat anything (adventurous consumers - AC) and only a small proportion that are DC.

Having a mixture of AC and DC individuals in a group could be a way for to adapt to changes in the availability of prey.

The researchers had already seen DC behavior in individuals, but the sociable stickleback is usually found in shoals. Being part of a group has obvious benefits - for example you're less likely to be picked off by a predator so have more time for eating. But there are also costs - like increased competition for food.

To test for DC in shoals, the researchers put 60 individual sticklebacks and 61 shoals (of three fish) into tanks and got them used to feeding on water fleas that had been coloured either brown or green. After a week, each fish was offered 19 of its familiar prey and one of the other, 'novel', colour (or 57 familiar and 3 novel fleas for each shoal). After an hour, all remaining prey were removed and counted.

The following day, the fish were offered the two colours in proportion to the numbers of each left uneaten the day before. So if more of the novel prey colour had survived, the fish would be given more of that color next time - effectively simulating how the two flea populations would breed in response to the pressure of predation by the .

For each tank the process continued until one flea color had reached 'fixation'- 100 per cent - and the other was extinct.

Theory would predict that fish in a shoal would be more likely to accept novel prey, because the individuals in the group have more time to get used to it and because the increased competition gives them more reason to eat it. And any shoal that includes AC individuals would be more likely to gobble up the novel prey while it's still rare.

But in this experiment, 15 per cent of the shoals were sufficiently DC to drive the novel flea to fixation, suggesting that the shoals acted more like DC individuals than AC individuals.

"The surprise result for us was not to find evidence that the individual fish were having a different effect to the shoals," says Thomas. "You would expect shoals to have at least one AC individual which should make them more likely to drive the novel prey extinct."

Scientists know that individual fish can learn from others in their group. In this case the results suggest that some fish that would otherwise eat anything have instead copied the DC behavior of their shoal mates. But the researchers don't yet know why.

"Dietary conservatism was discovered out of the blue," says Thomas. "This study focused on the selective pressure on , but there's lots more to find out. It would be good to look at the interactions between individual to see how they learn different behaviours from each other."


This story is republished courtesy of Planet Earth online, a free, companion website to the award-winning magazine Planet Earth published and funded by the Natural Environment Research Council (NERC).

Explore further: A step into the unmown creates a 'win-win' for wildlife and humans

More information: E Loys Richards, et al. The expression of dietary conservatism in solitary and shoaling 3-spined sticklebacks Gasterosteus aculeatus. Behavioral Ecology doi: 10/1093/beheco/arr047

Related Stories

Fish can be picky eaters

Jul 19, 2010

(PhysOrg.com) -- We all know how fussy kids can be about their food, but now new research suggests they're not the only ones.

Can angelfish do math?

Jan 27, 2011

(PhysOrg.com) -- Beauty and brains don't often come together. But angelfish, a species prized by pet fish owners for its bright good looks, might possess math skills, show studies coauthored by Professor Robert ...

Fish schools and krill swarms take on common shape

Sep 16, 2010

When fish or tiny, shrimp-like krill get together, it appears they follow the same set of "rules." According to a new study published online on September 16th in Current Biology, shoals of fish and swarms of krill hang o ...

Recommended for you

Dogs can be pessimists too

8 hours ago

Dogs generally seem to be cheerful, happy-go-lucky characters, so you might expect that most would have an optimistic outlook on life.

Transparent larvae hide opaque eyes behind reflections

21 hours ago

Becoming invisible is probably the ultimate form of camouflage: you don't just blend in, the background shows through you. And this strategy is not as uncommon as you might think. Kathryn Feller, from the University of Maryland ...

Peacock's train is not such a drag

22 hours ago

The magnificent plumage of the peacock may not be quite the sacrifice to love that it appears to be, University of Leeds researchers have discovered.

User comments : 0