Caterpillars aren't so bird brained after all

Apr 04, 2011

(PhysOrg.com) -- Caterpillars that masquerade as twigs to avoid becoming a bird's dinner are actually using clever behavioural strategies to outwit their predators, according to a new study.

Researchers at the universities of Exeter, Liverpool, Liverpool Hope and Glasgow have shown that when it comes to the art of , things are never quite the way they seem.

The study, published online today in the journal PNAS, reveals that twig-mimicking choose their location to maximise their chances of fooling their into thinking they are twigs.

Using domestic chicks (gallus gallus domesticus) as predators of twig mimicking Early Thorn caterpillars (selenia dentaria), research showed that caterpillars are more likely to fool when twigs are common.

In order to exploit birds' behaviour, caterpillars position themselves in locations where twigs are in abundance during the day – even if they're not good locations for feeding.

At night, when the predators cannot hunt by sight, the caterpillars go to rich feeding grounds – regardless of twig abundance.

Dr John Skelhorn, a lecturer in animal behaviour at the University of Exeter and lead author of the study, said: "The caterpillars are not just blindly mimicking inedible objects in the environment and hoping for the best, they are actually using complex habitat-selection strategies which exploit predators' hunting behaviour.

"In this study, we observed that the chicks were less likely to search for masquerading prey when the object they mimic, in this case twigs, is in abundance. It seems the low chances of finding prey and the amount of effort involved put them off.

"The caterpillars make the most of this by selecting habitats where twigs are common during the day, but then abandon them at night, when there are no visually-hunting predators around, in order to go in search of the best feeding grounds."

To come to this conclusion, the researchers carried out a number of different trials using the chicks and caterpillars. In the trials, the team found that chicks exposed to situations where they had little success in checking twigs became less likely to look in the first place. When prey was hidden amongst a higher number of twigs, it also took the much longer to find the caterpillars.

The research then looked at the behaviour of the caterpillars, placing them in an environment where they could choose between two branches – one with few twigs but plenty of food, and another with more twigs but no food.

Caterpillars showed a strong preference for the branch with lots of twigs, offering the best protection from their predators, during the daylight. At night, they opted for the food-rich branch.

Dr Skelhorn added: "This research shows that there are costs associated with masquerading as an inedible object. Caterpillars aren't free to go where they want and have to select their locations very carefully. The benefit of masquerade is also determined by how many twigs are around, and how many other individuals are masquerading as twigs in the local environment.

"If there are too many masqueraders in one area, predators will learn this and be highly motivated to search for them. It really relies on there being not too many individuals trying to pull the same trick.

Explore further: Male monkey filmed caring for dying mate (w/ Video)

Related Stories

Fat, thin caterpillars are studied

Sep 21, 2006

A U.S.-led international team of scientists says there's no obesity epidemic among insects and the researchers believe they now know why.

Insects that deter predators produce fewer offspring

Jan 25, 2011

Scientists studied the defences used by caterpillars that transform into large white butterflies, called Pieris brassicae. The insects regurgitate semi-digested cabbage leaves to make them smell and taste unpleasant to predators. ...

Beware: More caterpillars may be arriving

Nov 17, 2005

As global warming affects the Earth's climate, a Tulane University scientist is predicting a significant increase in caterpillars and agricultural damage.

Parasitoid turns its host into a bodyguard

Jun 04, 2008

There are many examples of parasites that induce spectacular changes in the behaviour of their host. Flukes, for example, are thought to induce ants, their intermediate host, to move up onto blades of grass during the night ...

Recommended for you

Male monkey filmed caring for dying mate (w/ Video)

Apr 18, 2014

(Phys.org) —The incident was captured by Dr Bruna Bezerra and colleagues in the Atlantic Forest in the Northeast of Brazil.  Dr Bezerra is a Research Associate at the University of Bristol and a Professor ...

Orchid named after UC Riverside researcher

Apr 17, 2014

One day about eight years ago, Katia Silvera, a postdoctoral scholar at the University of California, Riverside, and her father were on a field trip in a mountainous area in central Panama when they stumbled ...

In sex-reversed cave insects, females have the penises

Apr 17, 2014

Researchers reporting in the Cell Press journal Current Biology on April 17 have discovered little-known cave insects with rather novel sex lives. The Brazilian insects, which represent four distinct but re ...

Fear of the cuckoo mafia

Apr 17, 2014

If a restaurant owner fails to pay the protection money demanded of him, he can expect his premises to be trashed. Warnings like these are seldom required, however, as fear of the consequences is enough to ...

User comments : 0

More news stories

Biologists help solve fungi mysteries

(Phys.org) —A new genetic analysis revealing the previously unknown biodiversity and distribution of thousands of fungi in North America might also reveal a previously underappreciated contributor to climate ...

Making graphene in your kitchen

Graphene has been touted as a wonder material—the world's thinnest substance, but super-strong. Now scientists say it is so easy to make you could produce some in your kitchen.