The more you know a place, the more likely your memory will play spatial tricks

Jan 17, 2011
Using Northwestern University students, a new study shows that as students better understand the relationship between buildings on a campus, for example, over time memory biases cause them to exaggerate the distance between the north and south ends of campus.

(PhysOrg.com) -- Many suburbanites remember a time when they were once city dwellers. For a time many returned to the city for dining, cultural and entertainment purposes. But over time the suburbs and "the city" seemed much farther apart thereby resulting in less frequent trips.

A new Northwestern University study is the first to show that something may be happening cognitively that leads people to gradually become more biased, and at the same time more accurate, when it comes to their as they become more familiar with a particular area.

In other words, as people better understand the relationship between buildings on a campus, for example, over time biases cause them to exaggerate the distance between the north and south ends of campus. They become more and more biased and see the boundaries of campus as being much farther apart.

David Uttal, professor of in the Weinberg College of Arts and Sciences, along with colleague Alinda Friedman at the University of Alberta, are lead authors of the study. They witnessed firsthand how this plays out among students on the Northwestern campus.

“I’ve had students tell me that they may be a few minutes late for class because they are coming all the way from south campus,” Uttal said. “And I’m thinking, ‘It’s only a six-minute walk.’

“Another time I overheard a student say, ‘This better be good, because I don’t go to north campus for nothing.’

“That really intrigued me because if you look at a map it’s not at all clear where these divisions are,” said Uttal. “There are north and south ends of campus but treating it like a really sharp division, like a foreign world, it’s not justified based solely on geography. I think there is something really interesting going on here cognitively.”

There have been other studies demonstrating the existence of spatial biases and how they affect spatial judgments, but Uttal said there were never studies about how such biases developed and how they were learned.

By studying Northwestern freshmen over three quarters and comparing them to seniors, Uttal said two significant findings emerged.

First, Uttal said, “We can get simultaneously more accurate and more biased at the same time, which seems counterintuitive, but it really shows how different kinds of information are stored and thought about differently in the mind.”

Secondly, Uttal and his colleagues were able to establish a time-course for when these biases develop, concluding that when people are new to an area, they are not inherently biased. They develop biases as they become more familiar with their surroundings.

However, it is still unclear how people acquire information that lead to such biases.

“Part of it has to be somewhat cultural – so as you become more part of a group, a ‘northsider’ or ‘southsider’ if you will, you hear more about these distinctions,” Uttal said. “It’s also kind of an interaction between what you learn from other people and what you learn on your own.”

Uttal said the study’s findings have implications for both the Northwestern University community and society at large.

“One of the goals of our campus is to bring people closer together and establish community,” he said. “However, it is possible that people's cognitive biases could actually make this hard. If people start to think of different areas on campus as being farther apart as they get more involved in Northwestern, then it might actually be harder to get people to think about ‘One Northwestern’ the longer they are here.”

In addition, the study’s findings may have desegregation implications.

“As other researchers have shown, in segregated areas, people may tend to believe that they are farther apart than they really are,” Uttal said. “And if we’re trying to bring people together, we have to address the cognitive biases that they create. You tend to see the area that you’re close to as closer and the areas that are socially and cognitively further from you as being geographically farther. “

“Learning Fine-grained and Category Information in Navigable Real-World Space” was published in the journal Memory & Cognition in December. The study’s co-authors, in addition to Uttal and Friedman, are Linda Liu Hand and Christopher Warren, Northwestern University.

Explore further: Religious music brings benefit to seniors' mental health

Related Stories

Modern society made up of all types

Nov 04, 2010

Modern society has an intense interest in classifying people into ‘types’, according to a University of Melbourne Cultural Historian, leading to potentially catastrophic life-changing outcomes for those typed – ...

Study finds sick kids have fewer friends

Dec 07, 2010

A new study reveals that sick teens are more isolated than other kids, but they do not necessarily realize it and often think their friendships are stronger than they actually are.

Recommended for you

Religious music brings benefit to seniors' mental health

Apr 18, 2014

A new article published online in The Gerontologist reports that among older Christians, listening to religious music is associated with a decrease in anxiety about death and increases in life satisfaction, self-e ...

User comments : 6

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

Sackbut
not rated yet Jan 17, 2011
So where is the bias? The author says the participants become more accurate and show bias, but nowhere does the summary demonstrate this.
lexington
5 / 5 (1) Jan 17, 2011
These thing always bring up scary Lovecraft vibes in me. The idea that your mind is not your own, that it will screw you without you ever noticing, just terrifies me.
mysticfree
5 / 5 (2) Jan 17, 2011
Is it a simple matter that the more often you travel a given route, the more details of landmarks you remember? As you recall those details of your trip (without traveling), your estimation of the distance is bigger in comparison to when the trip was still a relatively new experience.
natetuvkok
not rated yet Jan 18, 2011
no it's a advertizing trip
being computerized the more u go to one place it will be catagorized sry bout the spellin tryin to be fast ha.
wileruilaer
5 / 5 (1) Jan 18, 2011
@mysticfree: I can only speak from personal experience, but I remember going far from home in my parents car when I was little (an hour away or more, an eternity) and because I could not memorize how the whole way was connected, it felt very long. As I later drove it myself and became familiar with it, and it's landmarks, it felt much shorter. I feel the same way about other routes nowaday. Long the first time, then successively shorter and shorter as i repeat them. So... opposite from what this article made you think?
CSharpner
5 / 5 (1) Jan 18, 2011
mysiticfree: That was my thought too.
Wileruilaer: I also thought that.

These two ideas seem to conflict, but I've experienced them both. Perhaps it's more than initial sense of distance is slowly changed. Maybe, the first trip does seem long because it's unfamiliar, but as soon as the 2nd or 3rd (or however many it takes to feel comfortable with it), it seems shorter, but then, over the long run, as you start learning more details of the trip, it slowly starts seeming longer.

Also, for the childhood rides, there's another thing going on there: Our sense of time as children vs. adulthood is vastly different. An hour to a child seems like an eternity, but to an adult, it can seem like minutes. I think boredom has a lot to do with that. A child in a car trip had little to do in the 1980's and earlier but look out the window at the same, monotonous scenery. Adults can keep their minds occupied with things they worry about, making time go by faster than when completely bored.

More news stories

Cancer stem cells linked to drug resistance

Most drugs used to treat lung, breast and pancreatic cancers also promote drug-resistance and ultimately spur tumor growth. Researchers at the University of California, San Diego School of Medicine have discovered ...

Making graphene in your kitchen

Graphene has been touted as a wonder material—the world's thinnest substance, but super-strong. Now scientists say it is so easy to make you could produce some in your kitchen.