Researchers find evidence of early marker for autism

Oct 13, 2010 by Diane Kukich
Photo by Ambre Alexander

Most health care professionals agree that early intervention is critical for the nearly 1 in 100 children now diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a range of complex neurodevelopment disorders characterized by social impairments, communication difficulties, and repetitive behavior patterns.

However, early intervention requires early diagnosis, and most children are between three and six years old before they are diagnosed with ASD.

Now, a team of researchers that includes Cole Galloway, associate professor in the Department of Physical Therapy at the University of Delaware, has found what seems to be an early marker for ASD -- infrequent gazing at other people when unprompted.

Galloway is co-author on a paper, with Anjana Bhat and Rebecca Landa, reporting the findings of a study carried out at the Kennedy Krieger Institute in Baltimore. Bhat earned her doctorate at Delaware under Galloway and went on to work as a postdoctoral research with Landa, director of the Center for Autism and Related Disorders at Kennedy Krieger. The paper was published in The Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology.

The study involved 50 six-month-old infants, 25 who have siblings with autism and are therefore considered high risk, and 25 from the general population, considered low risk.

In the experimental setup, the infants were seated in a custom chair with an attached joystick within easy reach, a musical toy located to the right, and their caregivers on the left. The researchers evaluated how quickly the infants learned that the joystick activated the toy, as well as their level of social engagement with their caregivers.

They found that the babies exhibited similar levels of social gazing when their caregivers actively engaged them by pointing at the toy or expressing excitement. However, the high-risk group spent less time looking to their caregivers and more time fixated on the toy or the joystick when the caregiver did not initiate contact.

This could indicate a disruption in development related to “joint attention,” which is often a core deficit for children with autism.

“We saw that simple learning was occurring in all of the babies,” Galloway says. “The high-risk infants learned as quickly as their peers that something happened when they pulled the joystick.”

“But the fact that these babies looked at Mom only when prompted by her, and not spontaneously, was an important finding with implications for their future social development. Take time to watch a baby without engaging her, and you'll see an amazing array of behaviors. One of those is that babies purposefully direct their looking to gain information as well as to initiate and maintain communication with you. One of the hallmarks of making friends is that you have to initiate contact -- kids can't just sit back and wait for others to approach them. Babies who lack initiation of gaze may not be noticed if the adults in their lives don't notice.”

According to Bhat, a key lesson learned in the study is that context is critical. “We have to study infants using a variety of contexts if we really want to learn what is going on with them,” she says.

Bhat, who did her doctoral work through UD's interdisciplinary Biomechanics and Movement Science (BIOMS) graduate program, says that the training she received in Galloway's lab prepared her for the multi-systems view needed to study problems like .

“Everything is related,” she says, “and we have to look at visual, motor, attentional, and social development together if we want an accurate picture of what is happening with babies this young.”

The subtle nature of the marker demands that clinicians actually understand the baby, Galloway says. But if the gaze test proves to be an effective diagnostic tool, it opens the door for the early intervention that can make a difference for these children socially.

Landa points out, for example, that electronic toys, which children can enjoy and operate without social engagement, could be replaced by simple songs paired with easy, predictable gestures to promote language and social learning.

Explore further: Impact of childhood bullying still evident after 40 years

add to favorites email to friend print save as pdf

Related Stories

Early exposure could prevent egg allergy in babies

Oct 04, 2010

(PhysOrg.com) -- Parents who delay giving their babies allergenic foods could be doing more harm than good, with a new Australian study showing the rate of egg allergy significantly increases among toddlers who are introduced ...

Babies learn to ride robots at UD

Nov 07, 2007

Babies driving robots. It sounds like the theme of a cartoon series but it is actually the focus of important and innovative research being conducted at the University of Delaware that could have significant ...

Babies & Robots: Infant power mobility on display

Feb 04, 2009

Children with mobility issues, like cerebral palsy and spina bifida, can't explore the world like other babies, because they can't crawl or walk. Infant development emerges from the thousands of daily discoveries ...

Robot Speaks the Language of Kids

Aug 05, 2010

(PhysOrg.com) -- Researchers are studying whether a small robot with a big personality holds the potential to help children with autism.

Recommended for you

Our brains are hardwired for language

11 hours ago

A groundbreaking study published in PLOS ONE by Prof. Iris Berent of Northeastern University and researchers at Harvard Medical School shows the brains of individual speakers are sensitive to language univer ...

Child burn effects far reaching for parents

16 hours ago

Parents of burn victims experience significant psychological distress for at least three months after the incident and may compromise the post-operative recovery of their child, WA research has found.

Internet use may cut retirees' depression

16 hours ago

Spending time online has the potential to ward off depression among retirees, particularly among those who live alone, according to research published online in The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences an ...

User comments : 0

More news stories

Turning off depression in the brain

Scientists have traced vulnerability to depression-like behaviors in mice to out-of-balance electrical activity inside neurons of the brain's reward circuit and experimentally reversed it – but there's ...

Scientists tether lionfish to Cayman reefs

Research done by U.S. scientists in the Cayman Islands suggests that native predators can be trained to gobble up invasive lionfish that colonize regional reefs and voraciously prey on juvenile marine creatures.

White House updating online privacy policy

A new Obama administration privacy policy out Friday explains how the government will gather the user data of online visitors to WhiteHouse.gov, mobile apps and social media sites. It also clarifies that ...

Six Nepalese dead, six missing in Everest avalanche

At least six Nepalese climbing guides have been killed and six others are missing after an avalanche struck Mount Everest early Friday in one of the deadliest accidents on the world's highest peak, officials ...